Pojdi na slovensko stran članka / Go to the article page in Slovene
Development of a computer-based visual attention training and investigation of its effectiveness in healthy adolescents
Lea Alič & Anja Podlesek
Full text (pdf) | Views: 67 | Written in Slovene. | Published: May 7, 2021
https://doi.org/10.20419/2021.30.531 | Cited By: CrossRef (0)
Abstract: Visual attention, an important cognitive function that helps select relevant information in the visual scene and fi lter out irrelevant information, can be trained and improved. We developed computer-based training of focused and distributed attention and of switching between global and local information processing and tested its eff ectiveness in healthy adolescents. In the experimental group (n = 11), training lasted 6 weeks, with three 20-min sessions per week. After each training session, participants received feedback on their performance and were able to track their progress. The control group (n = 10) received no intervention during this time. To test the eff ects of training, participants in both groups completed various cognitive tasks before and after training that shared features with the trained tasks to varying degrees. Compared to the control group, the experimental group showed greater improvements in reaction times on the selective attention and global-local processing tasks, while no consistent training eff ects were found on the other cognitive tests used. Thus, the results showed that the developed training can improve selected aspects of visual attention, but in line with most fi ndings on cognitive training in the literature, the eff ect is limited to improved performance on tasks that are very similar to the trained tasks.
Keywords: computer-based cognitive training, focused attention, distributed attention, global-local processing, transfer
Cite:
Alič, L., & Podlesek, A. (2021). Razvoj računalniškega treninga vidne pozornosti in preverjanje njegove učinkovitosti pri zdravih mladostnikih [Development of a computer-based visual attention training and investigation of its effectiveness in healthy adolescents]. Psihološka obzorja, 30, 112–128. https://doi.org/10.20419/2021.30.531
Reference list
Bazzaz, M. M., Fadardi, J. S., & Parkinson, J. (2017). Efficacy of the attention control training program on reducing attentional bias in obese and overweight dieters. Appetite, 108, 1–11. CrossRef
Bikic, A. (2016). Cognitive training in children and adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [neobjavljena doktorska disertacija]. Syddansk Universitet, Det Sundhedsvidenskabelige Fakultet.
Bogdanova, Y., Yee, M. K., Ho, V. T., & Cicerone, K. D. (2015). Computerized cognitive rehabilitation of attention and executive function in acquired brain injury: A systematic review. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 31(6), 419–433. CrossRef
Bosse, M. L., Tainturier, M. J., & Valdois, S. (2007). Developmental dyslexia: The visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition, 104(2), 198–230. CrossRef
Carrasco, M. (2011). Visual attention: The past 25 years. Vision Research, 51(13), 1484–1525. CrossRef
Chiu, H. L., Chu, H., Tsai, J. C., Liu, D., Chen, Y. R., Yang, H., & Chou, K. R. (2017). The effect of cognitive-based training for the healthy older people: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. PLOS ONE, 12(5), članek e0176742. CrossRef
Corsi, P. M. (1972). Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain [doktorska disertacija]. McGill University, Department of Psychology. Library and Archives Canada. https://central.bac-lac.gc.ca/.item?id=TC-QMM-70754&op=pdf&app=Library&oclc_number=895261380
Dulaney, C. L., & Marks, W. (2007). The effects of training and transfer on global/local processing. Acta Psychologica, 125(2), 203–220. CrossRef
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16(1), 143–149. CrossRef
Eriksen, C. W., & James, J. D. S. (1986). Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model. Perception & Psychophysics, 40(4), 225–240. CrossRef
Eriksen, C. W., & Schultz, D. W. (1979). Information processing in visual search: A continuous flow conception and experimental results. Perception & Psychophysics, 25(4), 249–263. CrossRef
Feng J., & Spence I. (2007) Effects of cognitive training on individual differences in attention. V D. Harris (ur.), Lecture Notes in Computer Science, vol 4562. Engineering Psychology and Cognitive Ergonomics. Springer. CrossRef
Feng, J., Spence, I., & Pratt, J. (2007). Playing an action video game reduces gender differences in spatial cognition. Psychological Science, 18(10), 850–855. CrossRef
Gazzaley, A., & Nobre, A. C. (2012). Top-down modulation: Bridging selective attention and working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 129–135. CrossRef
Greenwood, P. M., & Parasuraman, R. (2016). The mechanisms of far transfer from cognitive training: Review and hypothesis. Neuropsychology, 30(6), 742–755. CrossRef
Haeger, M., Bock, O., Memmert, D., & Hüttermann, S. (2018). Can driving-simulator training enhance visual attention, cognition, and physical functioning in older adults? Journal of Aging Research, 2018, članek 7547631. CrossRef
Halder, S., & Mahato, A. (2017). Efficacy of a model attention training program for children with ADHD. Journal of Indian Association for Child & Adolescent Mental Health, 13(1), 10–25.
Hanania, R., & Smith, L. B. (2010). Selective attention and attention switching: Towards a unified developmental approach. Developmental Science, 13(4), 622–635. CrossRef
Huettel, S. A., & McCarthy, G. (2004). What is odd in the oddball task? Prefrontal cortex is activated by dynamic changes in response strategy. Neuropsychologia, 42(3), 379–386. CrossRef
Hübner, R. (2014). Does attentional selectivity in global/local processing improve discretely or gradually? Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 61. CrossRef
Kueider, A. M., Parisi, J. M., Gross, A. L., & Rebok, G. W. (2012). Computerized cognitive training with older adults: A systematic review. PLOS ONE, 7(7), članek e40588. CrossRef
Lampit, A., Hallock, H., & Valenzuela, M. (2014). Computerized cognitive training in cognitively healthy older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis of effect modifiers. PLoS medicine, 11(11), članek e1001756. CrossRef
Lawrence, R. K., Edwards, M., & Goodhew, S. C. (2018). Changes in the spatial spread of attention with ageing. Acta Psychologica, 188, 188–199. CrossRef
Locke, H. S., & Braver, T. S. (2008). Motivational influences on cognitive control: Behavior, brain activation, and individual differences. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 8(1), 99–112. CrossRef
Mann, T. A., & Walker, P. (2003). Autism and a deficit in broadening the spread of visual attention. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(2), 274–284. CrossRef
Maraver, M. J., Bajo, M. T., & Gomez-Ariza, C. J. (2016). Training on working memory and inhibitory control in young adults. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 1–18. CrossRef
McMains, S. A., & Kastner, S. (2009). Visual Attention. V M. D. Binder, N. Hirokawa in U. Windhorst (ur.), Encyclopedia of neuroscience. Springer. CrossRef
Meng, F. W., Yao, Z. F., Chang, E. C., & Chen, Y. L. (2019). Team sport expertise shows superior stimulus-driven visual attention and motor inhibition. PLOS ONE, 14(5), članek e0217056. CrossRef
Moen, F., Firing, K., & Wells, A. (2016). The effects of attention training techniques on stress and performance in sports. International Journal of Applied Sports Sciences, 28(2), 213–225. CrossRef
Mozolic, J. L., Long, A. B., Morgan, A. R. Rawley-Payne, M., & Laurienti, P. J. (2011). A cognitive training intervention improves modality-specific attention in a randomized controlled trial of healthy older adults. Neurobiology of Aging, 32(4), 655–668. CrossRef
Mueller, S. T. (2014a). PEBL: The Psychology Experiment Building Language (Version 0.14) [programski jezik]. http://pebl.sourceforge.net. CrossRef
Mueller, S. T. (2014b). The PEBL manual: Programming and usage guide for the Psychology Experiment Building Language PEBL Version 0.14. http://pebl.sourceforge.net/peblmanual.pdfCrossRef
Mueller, S. T., & Piper, B. J. (2014). The Psychology Experiment Building Language (PEBL) and PEBL Test Battery. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 9, 222–250. CrossRef
Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees - Precedence of global features in visual-perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353–383. CrossRef
Oei, A. C., & Patterson, M. D. (2013). Enhancing cognition with video games: A multiple game training study. PLPS ONE, 8(3), članek e58546. CrossRef
Peirce, J. W. (2008). Generating stimuli for neuroscience using PsychoPy. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics, 2, 10. CrossRef
Peng, P., & Miller, A. C. (2016). Does attention training work? A selective meta-analysis to explore the effects of attention training and moderators. Learning and Individual Differences, 45, 77–87. CrossRef
Perkins, D. N., & Salomon, G. (1992). Transfer of learning. V T. Husén in T. N. Postlethwaite (ur.), International encyclopedia of education (2. izd.). Pergamon Press. https://www.researchgate.net/file.PostFileLoader.html?id=539ccac5d039b1b6438b460b&assetKey=AS%3A273546285125640%401442229925004
Rabiner, D. L., Murray, D. W., Skinner, A. T., & Malone, P. S. (2010). A randomized trial of two promising computer-based interventions for students with attention difficulties. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38(1), 131–142. CrossRef
Richardson, E. D., & Marottoli, R. A. (2003). Visual attention and driving behaviors among community-living older persons. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 58(9), M832–M836. CrossRef
Rueda, M. R., Checa, P., & Cómbita, L. M. (2012). Enhanced efficiency of the executive attention network after training in preschool children: Immediate changes and effects after two months. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2(1), 192–204. CrossRef
Sala, G., Aksayli, N., Tatlıdil, S., Tatsumi, T., Gondo, Y., & Gobet, F. (2018). Near and far transfer in cognitive training: A second-order meta-analysis. Collabra: Psychology, 5(1), 18. CrossRef
Simons, D. J., Boot, W. R., Charness, N., Gathercole, S. E., Chabris, C. F., Hambrick, D. Z., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. (2016). Do "brain-training" programs work? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 17(3), 103–186. CrossRef
Solan, H. A., Larson, S., Shelley-Tremblay, J., Ficarra, A., & Silverman, M. (2001). Role of visual attention in cognitive control of oculomotor readiness in students with reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34(2), 107–118. CrossRef
Steiner, N. J., Sheldrick, R. C., Gotthelf, D., & Perrin, E. C. (2011). Computer-based attention training in the schools for children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A preliminary trial. Clinical Pediatrics, 50(7), 615–622. CrossRef
Tamm, L., Epstein, J. N., Peugh, J. L., Nakonezny, P. A., & Hughes, C. W. (2013). Preliminary data suggesting the efficacy of attention training for school-aged children with ADHD. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 16–28. CrossRef
Treisman, A. (2006). How the deployment of attention determines what we see. Visual Cognition, 14(4–8), 411–443. CrossRef
Valdois, S., Roulin, J. L., & Bosse, M. L. (2019). Visual attention modulates reading acquisition. Vision Research, 165, 152–161. CrossRef
Veloso, A., Vicente, S. G., & Filipe, M. G. (2020). Effectiveness of cognitive training for school-aged children and adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2983. CrossRef
Wang, P., Liu, H. H., Zhu, X. T., Meng, T., Li, H. J., & Zuo, X. N. (2016). Action video game training for healthy adults: A meta-analytic study. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 907. CrossRef
Wang, Z., Zhou, R., & Shah, P. (2014). Spaced cognitive training promotes training transfer. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 217. CrossRef
Wass, S., Scerif, G., & Johnson, M. (2012). Training attentional control and working memory - Is younger, better? Developmental Review, 32(4), 360–387. CrossRef