« Nazaj na Letnik 21 (2012), Številka 1
Go to the article page in English / Pojdi na angleško stran članka
Povezanost teorije uma s socialnimi odnosi in otrokovo socialno kompetentnostjo
Nuša Skubic
Polno besedilo (pdf) | Ogledi: 152 | Napisan v slovenščini. | Objavljeno: 28. oktober 2012
https://doi.org/10.20419/2012.21.358 | Citati: CrossRef (0)
Povzetek: Prispevek predstavlja pregled in evalvacijo ugotovitev raziskovalcev s področja teorije uma v povezavi s socialnimi odnosi in na drugi strani v povezavi z otrokovo socialno kompetentnostjo, ki je odraz njegove teorije uma. Prikazuje, kateri so tisti dejavniki, ki največ prispevajo k individualnim razlikam v razvitosti teorije uma med otroki in poudarja dvosmernost učinkov med otrokovim socialnim razumevanjem in socialnimi odnosi z drugimi. Pozitivni dejavniki za razvoj otrokove teorije uma so v prvi vrsti otrokove zgodnje kakovostne izkušnje o mentalnih stanjih, ki dobro napovedujejo njegov poznejši dosežek pri preizkusu razumevanja napačnih prepričanj. Od družinskih dejavnikov so za razvoj otrokove teorije uma pomembni predvsem socialno-ekonomski položaj družine, vedenje in govor staršev (npr. ustrezna raba izrazov za opisovanje mentalnih stanj in ustrezni načini discipliniranja otroka) in prisotnost ustrezno starega (ponavadi starejšega) sorojenca, s katerim ima otrok kakovosten odnos, medtem ko raziskovalci izmed dejavnikov, ki niso vezani na družinsko okolje, najbolj poudarjajo pomen vrstnikov za otrokovo teorijo uma. Skladno s temi dejavniki se pri otroku bolj ali manj uspešno razvija njegovo socialno razumevanje, ki igra pomembno vlogo v njegovem vsakdanjem življenju. Tako otroci z dobro razvito teorijo uma to lahko uporabljajo na prosocialen način, lahko pa ta služi tudi proaktivni oziroma posredni obliki agresivnosti, ko otroci svoje razumevanje drugih uporabljajo za različne oblike manipulacij oz. nasilništva, posebej v vrstniški skupini. Nizko razvita teorija uma je dejavnik tveganja predvsem v primeru neugodnih družinskih razmer, medtem ko ima dobro razvita teorija uma v takih razmerah za otrokov razvoj lahko zaščitno vlogo. Prispevek opozarja na nekatere metodološke in strukturne pomanjkljivosti pregledanih študij ter predlaga usmeritve za možnosti celovitejšega raziskovanja otrokove teorije uma v prihodnje.
Ključne besede: teorija uma, socialni odnosi, socialna kompetentnost, socialno vedenje, Vygotsky, agresivnost
Citiraj:
Skubic, N. (2012). Povezanost teorije uma s socialnimi odnosi in otrokovo socialno kompetentnostjo [Association of theory of mind with social relations and child's social competence]. Psihološka obzorja, 21(1), 51–66. https://doi.org/10.20419/2012.21.358
Seznam literature v članku
Badenes, L. V., Estevan, R. A. C. in Bacete, F. J. (2000). Theory of mind and peer rejection at school. Social Development, 9(3), 271–283. CrossRef
Brown, J. R., Donelan-McCall, N. in Dunn, J. (1996). Why talk about mental states? The significance of children's conversations with friends, siblings, and mothers. Child Development, 67(3), 836–849. CrossRef
Caravita, S. C., Di Blasio, P. in Salmivalli, C. (2010). Early adolescents' participation in bullying: Is ToM involved? The Journal of Early Adolescence, 30(1), 138–170. CrossRef
Carpendale, J. in Lewis, C. (2006a). Social interaction and the development of social understanding: The role of relationships in social cognitive development. V J. Carpendale in C. Lewis (ur.), How children develop social understanding (str. 133–157). Oxford: Blackwell publishing. >
Carpendale, J. in Lewis, C. (2006b). Social understanding and children's social lives. V J. Carpendale in C. Lewis (ur.), How children develop social understanding (str. 205–230). Oxford: Blackwell publishing. >
Carpendale, J. I. in Lewis, C. (2004). Constructing an understanding of mind: The development of children's social understanding within social interaction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(1), 79–151. CrossRef
Cassidy, K. W., Fineberg, D. S., Brown, K. in Perkins, A. (2005). Theory of mind may be contagious, but you don't catch it from your twin. Child Development, 76(1), 97–106. CrossRef
Cassidy, K. W., Werner, R. S., Rourke, M., Zubernis, L. S. in Balaraman, G. (2003). The relationship between psychological understanding and positive social behaviors. Social Development, 12(2), 198–221. CrossRef
Crick, N. R. in Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social information-processing mechanisms on reactive and proactive aggression. Child Development, 67(3), 993–1002. CrossRef
Cutting, A. L. in Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind, emotion understanding, language, and family background: Individual differences and interrelations. Child Development, 70(4), 853–865. CrossRef
de Rosnay, M. in Hughes, C. (2006). Conversation and theory of mind: Do children talk their way to socio-cognitive understanding? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(1), 7–37. CrossRef
Fahie, C. M. in Symons, D. K. (2003). Executive functioning and theory of mind in children clinically referred for attention and behavior problems. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 24(1), 51–73. CrossRef
Fernyhough, C. (2008). Getting Vygotskian about theory of mind: Mediation, dialogue, and the development of social understanding. Developmental Review, 28(2), 225–262. CrossRef
Foote, R. C. in Holmes-Lonergan, H. A. (2003). Sibling conflict and theory of mind. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21(1), 45–58. CrossRef
Howe, N., Rinaldi, C. M., Jennings, M. in Petrakos, H. (2002). "No! The lambs can stay out because they got cozies!": Constructive and destructive sibling conflict, pretend play, and social understanding. Child Development, 73(5), 1460–1473. CrossRef
Hughes, C. in Cutting, A. L. (1999). Nature, nurture, and individual differences in early understanding of mind. Psychological Science, 10(5), 429–432. CrossRef
Hughes, C., Deater-Deckard, K. in Cutting, A. L. (1999). "Speak roughly to your little boy?" Sex differences in the relations between parenting and preschoolers' understanding of mind. Social Development, 8(2), 143–160. CrossRef
Hughes, C. in Ensor, R. (2005). Executive function and theory of mind in 2 year olds: A family affair? Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 645–668. CrossRef
Hughes, C. in Ensor, R. (2006). Behavioural problems in 2-year-olds: Links with individual differences in theory of mind, executive function and harsh parenting. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(5), 488–497. CrossRef
Hughes, C. in Ensor, R. (2007). Positive and protective: Effects of early theory of mind on problem behaviors in at-risk preschoolers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(10), 1025–1032. CrossRef
Hughes, C., Fujisawa, K. K., Ensor, R., Lecce, S. in Marfleet, R. (2006). Cooperation and conversations about the mind: A study of individual differences in 2-year-olds and their siblings. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(1), 53–72. CrossRef
Hughes, C., Jaffee, S. R., Happe, F., Taylor, A., Caspi, A. in Moffitt, T. E. (2005). Origins of individual differences in theory of mind: From nature to nurture? Child Development, 76(2), 356–370. CrossRef
Jenkins, J. M. in Astington, J. W. (1996). Cognitive factors and family structure associated with theory of mind development in young children. Developmental Psychology, 32(1), 70–78. CrossRef
Kavčič, T. in Zupančič, M. (2006). Parental differential treatment of siblings in childhood. Psihološka Obzorja/Horizons of Psychology, 15(4), 5–24.
Lane, J. D., Wellman, H. M., Olson, S. L., LaBounty, J. in Kerr, D. C. (2010). Theory of mind and emotion understanding predict moral development in early childhood. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28(4), 871–889. CrossRef
Lewis, C., Freeman, N. H., Kyriakidou, C., Maridaki-Kassotaki, K. in Berridge, D. M. (1996). Social influences on false belief access: Specific sibling influences or general apprenticeship? Child Development, 67(6), 2930–2947. CrossRef
Marjanovič Umek, L. (2009). Spoznavni razvoj v zgodnjem otroštvu [Cognitive development in early choldhood]. V L. Marjanovič Umek in M. Zupančič (ur.), Razvojna psihologija [Developmental psychology] (str. 291–314). Ljubljana: Znanstvena založba Filozofske fakultete.
Marjanovič Umek, L., Zupančič, M., Fekonja, U., Lešnik Musek, P. in Kavčič, T. (2001). Socialni razvoj v zgodnjem otroštvu in teorija uma [Social development in early childhood and theory of mind]. V L. Marjanovič Umek in M. Zupančič (ur.), Razvojna psihologija: izbrane teme [Developmental psychology: Selected works] (str. 42–59). Ljubljana: Oddelek za psihologijo Filozofske fakultete.
McAlister, A. in Peterson, C. (2007). A longitudinal study of child siblings and theory of mind development. Cognitive Development, 22(2), 258–270. CrossRef
McAlister, A. in Peterson, C. C. (2006). Mental playmates: Siblings, executive functioning and theory of mind. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(4), 733–751. CrossRef
McElwain, N. L. in Volling, B. L. (2004). Attachment security and parental sensitivity during infancy: Associations with friendship quality and false-belief understanding at age 4. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 21(5), 639–667. CrossRef
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Russell, J. in Clark-Carter, D. (1998). Security of attachment as a predictor of symbolic and mentalising abilities: A longitudinal study. Social Development, 7(1), 1–24. CrossRef
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Gupta, M. D., Fradley, E. in Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Development, 73(6), 1715–1726. CrossRef
Ontai, L. L. in Thompson, R. A. (2008). Attachment, parent-child discourse and theory-of-mind development. Social Development, 17(1), 47–60. CrossRef
Pears, K. C. in Moses, L. J. (2003). Demographics, parenting, and theory of mind in preschool children. Social Development, 12(1), 1–19. CrossRef
Perner, J., Ruffman, T. in Leekam, S. R. (1994). Theory of mind is contagious: You catch it from your sibs. Child Development, 65(4), 1228–1238. CrossRef
Peterson, C. C. (2000). Influence of siblings' perspectives on theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15(4), 435–455. CrossRef
Peterson, C. C., Garnett, M., Kelly, A. in Attwood, T. (2009). Everyday social and conversation applications of theory-of-mind understanding by children with autism-spectrum disorders or typical development. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 18(2), 105–115. CrossRef
Peterson, C. C. in Siegal, M. (2002). Mindreading and moral awareness in popular and rejected preschoolers. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20(2), 205–224. CrossRef
Racine, T. P., Carpendale, J. I. in Turnbull, W. (2006). Cross-sectional and longitudinal relations between mother-child talk about conflict and children's social understanding. British Journal of Psychology, 97(4), 521–536. CrossRef
Randell, A. C. in Peterson, C. C. (2009). Affective qualities of sibling disputes, mothers' conflict attitudes, and children's theory of mind development. Social Development, 18(4), 857–874. CrossRef
Razza, R. A. in Blair, C. (2009). Associations among false-belief understanding, executive function, and social competence: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30(3), 332–343. CrossRef
Recchia, H. E. in Howe, N. (2009). Associations between social understanding, sibling relationship quality, and siblings' conflict strategies and outcomes. Child Development, 80(5), 1564–1578. CrossRef
Renouf, A., Brendgen, M., Parent, S., Vitaro, F., Zelazo, P. D., Boivin, M., Dionne, G., Tremblay, R. E., Perusse, D. in Seguin, J. R. (2010a). Relations between theory of mind and indirect and physical aggression in kindergarten: Evidence of the moderating role of prosocial behaviors. Social Development, 19(3), 535–555. CrossRef
Renouf, A., Brendgen, M., Seguin, J. R., Vitaro, F., Boivin, M., Dionne, G., Tremblay, R. E. in Perusse, D. (2010b). Interactive links between theory of mind, peer victimization, and reactive and proactive aggression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology: An official publication of the International Society for Research in Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 38(8), 1109–1123. CrossRef
Ronald, A., Happe, F., Hughes, C. in Plomin, R. (2005). Nice and nasty theory of mind in preschool children: Nature and nurture. Social Development, 14(4), 664–684. CrossRef
Ronald, A., Viding, E., Happe, F. in Plomin, R. (2006). Individual differences in theory of mind ability in middle childhood and links with verbal ability and autistic traits: A twin study. Social Neuroscience, 1(3–4) , 412–425. CrossRef
Ruffman, T., Perner, J., Naito, M., Parkin, L. in Clements, W. A. (1998). Older (but not younger) siblings facilitate false belief understanding. Developmental Psychology, 34(1), 161–174. CrossRef
Ruffman, T., Perner, J. in Parkin, L. (1999). How parenting style affects false belief understanding. Social Development, 8(3), 395–411. CrossRef
Ruffman, T., Slade, L., Devitt, K. in Crowe, E. (2006). What mothers say and what they do: The relation between parenting, theory of mind, language and conflict/cooperation. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(1), 105–124. CrossRef
Runions, K. C. in Keating, D. P. (2007). Young children's social information processing: Family antecedents and behavioral correlates. Developmental Psychology, 43(4), 838–849. CrossRef
Sabbagh, M. A. in Callanan, M. A. (1998). Metarepresentation in action: 3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds' developing theories of mind in parent-child conversations. Developmental Psychology, 34(3), 491–502. CrossRef
Slaughter, V., Dennis, M. J. in Pritchard, M. (2002). Theory of mind and peer acceptance in preschool children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20(4), 545–564. CrossRef
Slaughter, V., Peterson, C. C. in Mackintosh, E. (2007). Mind what mother says: Narrative input and theory of mind in typical children and those on the autism spectrum. Child Development, 78(3), 839–858. CrossRef
Sutton, J., Smith, P. K. in Swettenham, J. (1999). Bullying and "theory of mind': A critique of the "social skills deficit' view of anti-social behaviour. Social Development, 8(1), 117–127. CrossRef
Symons, D. K. in Clark, S. E. (2000). A longitudinal study of mother-child relationships and theory of mind in the preschool period. Social Development, 9(1), 3–23. CrossRef
Symons, D. K., Fossum, K.-L. M. in Collins, T. (2006). A longitudinal study of belief and desire state discourse during mother-child play and later false belief understanding. Social Development, 15(4), 676–691. CrossRef
Symons, D. K., Peterson, C. C., Slaughter, V., Roche, J. in Doyle, E. (2005). Theory of mind and mental state discourse during book reading and story-telling tasks. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23(1), 81–102. CrossRef
Vygotsky, L. S. (2010). Mišljenje in govor [Thought and Language] . Ljubljana: Pedagoška fakulteta.
Walker, S. (2005). Gender differences in the relationship between young children's peer-related social competence and individual differences in theory of mind. The Journal of Genetic Psychology: Research and Theory on Human Development, 166(3), 297–312. CrossRef
Wang, Y. in Su, Y. (2009). False belief understanding: Children catch it from classmates of different ages. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 33(4), 331–336. CrossRef
Watson, A. C., Nixon, C. L., Wilson, A. in Capage, L. (1999). Social interaction skills and theory of mind in young children. Developmental Psychology, 35(2), 386–391. CrossRef
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D. in Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655–684. CrossRef
Woolfe, T., Want, S. C. in Siegal, M. (2002). Signposts to development: Theory of mind in deaf children. Child Development, 73(3), 768–778. CrossRef
Yagmurlu, B., Berument, S. K. in Celimli, S. (2005). The role of institution and home contexts in theory of mind development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 26(5), 521–537. CrossRef