Spletna stran Psiholoških obzorij uporablja piškotke za namene avtentikacije uporabnikov po prijavi na spletno stran, morebitno stalno prijavo na željo uporabnika in za namen beleženja števila ogledov posameznih strani Psiholoških obzorij.
Ali se strinjate, da na vaš računalnik (brskalnik) naložimo piškotke za te namene? Svojo odločitev lahko kasneje tudi spremenite na strani Zasebnost.

Želim izvedeti več

Psihološka obzorja :: Horizons of Psychology

Znanstveno-strokovna psihološka revija Društva psihologov Slovenije

Indeksirana v:
Scopus
PsycINFO
Academic OneFile

Smo člani DOAJ in CrossRef

sien
VSEBINA ZA AVTORJE PREDSTAVITEV UREDNIŠTVO POVEZAVE

Iskalnik

Moj račun

Članki z največ ogledi

 

« Nazaj na Letnik 31 (2022)

flag Go to the article page in English / Pojdi na angleško stran članka


Kaj se lahko naučimo od Jacquesa Mehlerja, klasičnega kognitivnega znanstvenika

Amanda Saksida

pdf Polno besedilo (pdf)  |  Ogledi: 193  |  flagNapisan v slovenščini.  |  Objavljeno: 28. januar 2022

pdf https://doi.org/10.20419/2022.31.545  |  Citati: CrossRef (0)

Povzetek: Prispevek prikazuje življenjsko delo enega od akterjev tako imenovane kognitivne revolucije, Jacquesa Mehlerja, ki je bil eden uspešnejših evropskih raziskovalcev razvoja človeške kognicije, še posebej razvoja govora. Ob tem članek predstavi glavne predpostavke klasične kognitivne znanosti – modularnost uma ter vlogo narave in vzgoje pri razvoju in delovanju miselnih procesov – in opiše, katere vpoglede v delovanje uma je omogočilo njegovo empirično raziskovanje v preteklih desetletjih. Na kratko tudi predstavi nova spoznanja, ki so kognitivno znanost v zadnjih dveh desetletjih dodobra spremenila in ki so deloma vplivala tudi na njegovo delo. Način, kako je Mehler ta nova spoznanja vedno znova integriral v svoje delo, lahko predstavlja enega od modelov sinteze empiričnega in teoretskega raziskovanja.

Ključne besede: klasična kognitivna znanost, modularnost uma, razvoj govora, Jacques Mehler


Citiraj:
Saksida, A. (2022). Kaj se lahko naučimo od Jacquesa Mehlerja, klasičnega kognitivnega znanstvenika [What can we learn from Jacques Mehler, a classical cognitive scientist]. Psihološka obzorja, 31, 1–7. https://doi.org/10.20419/2022.31.545


Seznam literature v članku


Anderson, M. L. (2010). Neural reuse: A fundamental organizational principle of the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 245–313. CrossRef

Anderson, M. L. in Finlay, B. L. (2014). Allocating structure to function: The strong links between neuroplasticity and natural selection. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, članek 918. CrossRef

Baddeley, A. D. (2017). Modularity, working memory and language acquisition. Second Language Research, 33(3), 299–311. CrossRef

Barrett, H. C. (2005). Enzymatic computation and cognitive modularity. Mind in Language, 20(3), 259–287. CrossRef

Benavides-Varela, S., Hochmann, J.-R., Macagno, F., Nespor, M. in Mehler, J. (2012). Newborn's brain activity signals the origin of word memories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(44), 17908–17913. CrossRef

Benavides-Varela, S. in Mehler, J. (2015). Verbal positional memory in 7-month-olds. Child Development, 86(1), 209–223. CrossRef

Benavides-Varela, S., Siugzdaite, R., Gómez, D. M., Macagno, F., Cattarossi, L. in Mehler, J. (2017). Brain regions and functional interactions supporting early word recognition in the face of input variability. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(29), 7588–7593. CrossRef

Bertoncini, J., Floccia, C., Nazzi, T. in Mehler, J. (1995). Morae and syllables: Rhythmical basis of speech representations in neonates. Language and Speech, 38(4), 311–329. CrossRef

Bertoncini, J. in Mehler, J. (1981). Syllables as units in infant speech perception. Infant Behavior and Development, 4, 247–260. CrossRef

Bonatti, L. L., Peña, M., Nespor, M. in Mehler, J. (2005). Linguistic constraints on statistical computations. Psychological Science, 16(6), 451–459. CrossRef

Bonatti, L. L., Peña, M., Nespor, M. in Mehler, J. (2007). On consonants, vowels, chickens, and eggs. Psychological Science, 18(10), 924–925. CrossRef

Chomsky, N. (1959). A review of B. F. Skinner's verbal behavior. Language, 1, 26–58. CrossRef

Christophe, A., Dupoux, E., Bertoncini, J. in Mehler, J. (1994). Do infants perceive word boundaries? An empirical study of the bootstrapping of lexical acquisition. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 95(3), 1570–1580. CrossRef

Christophe, A., Mehler, J. in Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2001). Perception of prosodic boundary correlates by newborn infants. Infancy, 2(3), 385–394. CrossRef

Cutler, A. in Mehler, J. (1993). The periodicity bias. Journal of Phonetics, 21(1/2), 103–108. CrossRef

Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D. in Segui, J. (1986). The syllable's differing role in the segmentation of French and English. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 385–400. CrossRef

Dehaene, S. (2004). Evolution of human cortical circuits for reading and arithmetic: The "neuronal recycling" hypothesis. V S. Dehaene, R. Duhamel, M. Hauser in G. Rizzolatti (ur.), From monkey brain to human brain (str. 133–157). MIT Press. CrossRef

Dehaene, S., Tzourio, N., Frak, V., Raynaud, L., Cohen, L., Mehler, J. in Mazoyer, B. (1996). Cerebral activations during number multiplication and comparison: A PET study. Neuropsychologia, 34(11), 1097–1106. CrossRef

Donovan, T., Dunn, K., Reid, V. M., Penman, A. in Young, R. J. (2020). Fetal eye movements in response to a visual stimulus. Brain and Behavior, 10(8), članek e01676. CrossRef

Elliott, J. G. in Grigorenko, E. L. (2014). The dyslexia debate. Cambridge University Press. CrossRef

Endress, A. D., Dehaene-Lambertz, G. in Mehler, J. (2007). Perceptual constraints and the learnability of simple grammars. Cognition, 105(3), 577–614. CrossRef

Endress, A. D. in Hauser, M. D. (2009). Syntax-induced pattern deafness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(49), 21001–21006. CrossRef

Endress, A. D. in Mehler, J. (2009). The surprising power of statistical learning: When fragment knowledge leads to false memories of unheard words. Journal of Memory and Language, 60(3), 351–367. CrossRef

Endress, A. D., Nespor, M. in Mehler, J. (2009). Perceptual and memory constraints on language acquisition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(8), 348–353. CrossRef

Endress, A. D., Scholl, B. J. in Mehler, J. (2005). The role of salience in the extraction of algebraic rules. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(3), 406–419. CrossRef

Ferry, A. L., Fló, A., Brusini, P., Cattarossi, L., Macagno, F., Nespor, M. in Mehler, J. (2016). On the edge of language acquisition: Inherent constraints on encoding multisyllabic sequences in the neonate brain. Developmental Science, 19(3), 488–503. CrossRef

Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. MIT Press. CrossRef

Gervain, J., Macagno, F., Cogoi, S., Peña, M. in Mehler, J. (2008). The neonate brain detects speech structure. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(37), 14222–14227. CrossRef

Gibson, E. in Wexler, K. (1994). Triggers. Linguistic Inquiry, 25(3), 407–454.

Goswami, U. (2015). Sensory theories of developmental dyslexia: Three challenges for research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(1), 43–54. CrossRef

Granier-Deferre, C., Bassereau, S., Ribeiro, A., Jacquet, A.-Y. in DeCasper, A. J. (2011). A melodic contour repeatedly experienced by human near-term fetuses elicits a profound cardiac reaction one month after birth. PLOS ONE, 6(2), članek e17304. CrossRef

Grayson, D. S., Ray, S., Carpenter, S., Iyer, S., Costa Dias, T. G., Stevens, C., Nigg, J. T. in Fair, D. A. (2014). Structural and functional rich club organization of the brain in children and adults. PLOS ONE, 9(2), članek e88297. CrossRef

Guida, A., Campitelli, G. in Gobet, F. (2016). Becoming an expert: Ontogeny of expertise as an example of neural reuse. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, članek e123. CrossRef

Hari, R. in Renvall, H. (2001). Impaired processing of rapid stimulus sequences in dyslexia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(12), 525–532. CrossRef

Helenius, P., Uutela, K. in Hari, R. (1999). Auditory stream segregation in dyslexic adults. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 122(5), 907–913. CrossRef

Hochmann, J.-R., Azadpour, M. in Mehler, J. (2008). Do humans really learn AnBn artificial grammars from exemplars? Cognitive Science, 32(6), 1021–1036. CrossRef

Hochmann, J.-R., Benavides-Varela, S., Nespor, M. in Mehler, J. (2011). Consonants and vowels: Different roles in early language acquisition. Developmental Science, 14(6), 1445–1458. CrossRef

Izard, V., Sann, C., Spelke, E. S. in Streri, A. (2009). Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(25), 10382–10385. CrossRef

Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1998). Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(10), 389–398. CrossRef

Kovács, A. M. in Mehler, J. (2009a). Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(16), 6556–6560. CrossRef

Kovács, A. M. in Mehler, J. (2009b). Flexible learning of multiple speech structures in bilingual infants. Science, 325(5940), 611–612. CrossRef

Mahmoudzadeh, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Fournier, M., Kongolo, G., Goudjil, S., Dubois, J., Grebe, R. in Wallois, F. (2013). Syllabic discrimination in premature human infants prior to complete formation of cortical layers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(12), 4846–4851. CrossRef

Marcus, G. F., Vijayan, S., Bandi Rao, S. in Vishton, P. M. (1999). Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. Science, 283(5398), 77–80. CrossRef

Mehler, J., Dommergues, J. Y., Frauenfelder, U. in Segui, J. (1981). The syllable's role in speech segmentation. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 298–305. CrossRef

Mehler, J., Jusczyk, P., Lambertz, G., Halsted, N., Bertoncini, J. in Amiel-Tison, C. (1988). A precursor of language acquisition in young infants. Cognition, 29(2), 143–178. CrossRef

Mehler, J., Peña, M., Nespor, M. in Bonatti, L. (2006). The "soul" of language does not use statistics: Reflections on vowels and consonants. Cortex, 4(42), 846–854. CrossRef

Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J. in Mehler, J. (1998). Language discrimination by newborns: Toward an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 756–766. CrossRef

Nespor, M., Peña, M. in Mehler, J. (2003). On the different roles of vowels and consonants in speech processing and language acquisition. Lingue e Linguaggio II, 203–230. ">CrossRef

Pallier, C., Dehaene, S., Poline, J., Lebihan, D., Argenti, A. M., Dupoux, E. in Mehler, J. (2003). Brain imaging of language plasticity in adopted adults: Can a second language replace the first? Cerebral Cortex, 13, 155–161. CrossRef

Panneton, R., Bremner, J. G. in Johnson, S. P. (2020). Infancy studies come of age: Jacques Mehler's influence on the importance of perinatal experience for early language learning. Cognition, 213, članek 104543. CrossRef

Pascual-Leone, A. in Hamilton, R. (2001). The metamodal organization of the brain. Progress in Brain Research, 134, 425–445. CrossRef

Peña, M., Bonatti, L. L., Nespor, M. in Mehler, J. (2002). Signal-driven computations in speech processing. Science, 298(5593), 604–607. CrossRef

Peña, M., Maki, A., Kovačić, D., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Koizumi, H., Bouquet, F. in Mehler, J. (2003). Sounds and silence: An optical topography study of language recognition at birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(20), 11702–11705. CrossRef

Perani, D., Dehaene, S., Grassi, F., Cohen, L., Cappa, S. F., Dupoux, E., Fazio, F. in Mehler, J. (1996). Brain processing of native and foreign languages. Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuropsychology, 7, 2439–2444. CrossRef

Perani, D., Paulesu, E., Galles, N. S., Dupoux, E., Dehaene, S., Bettinardi, V., Cappa, S. F., Fazio, F. in Mehler, J. (1998). The bilingual brain proficiency and age of acquisition of the second language. Brain, 121, 1841–1852. CrossRef

Peretz, I., Ayotte, J., Zatorre, R. J., Mehler, J., Ahad, P., Penhune, V. B. in Hw, Q. (2002). Congenital amusia: A disorder of fine-grained pitch discrimination. Neuron, 33, 185–191. CrossRef

Peretz, I. in Coltheart, M. (2015). Modularity of music processing. Nature Neuroscience, 6(7), 688–691. CrossRef

Piattelli-Palmarini, M. (1981). Language and learning: The debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky. Language, 57(4), 948–953. CrossRef

Piattelli-Palmarini, M. (2001). Portrait of a "classical" cognitive scientist: What I have learned from Jacques Mehler. V M. Dupoux (ur.), Language, brain, and cognitive development: Essays in Honor of Jacques Mehler (str. 3–21). The MIT Press.

Ramus, F., Hauser, M. D., Miller, C., Morris, D. in Mehler, J. (2000). Language discrimination by human newborns and by cotton-top tamarin monkeys. Science, 288(5464), 349–351. CrossRef

Ramus, F. in Szenkovits, G. (2008). What phonological deficit? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(1), 129–141. CrossRef

Reid, V. M., Dunn, K., Young, R. J., Amu, J., Donovan, T. in Reissland, N. (2017). The human fetus preferentially engages with face-like visual stimuli. Current Biology, 27(12), 1825–1828. CrossRef

Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N. in Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274(5294), 1926–1928. CrossRef

Saffran, J. R., Newport, E. L. in Aslin, R. N. (1996). Word segmentation: The role of fistributional cues. Journal of Memory and Language, 621(35), 606–621. CrossRef

Saksida, A., Langus, A. in Nespor, M. (2017). Co-occurrence statistics as a language-dependent cue for speech segmentation. Developmental Science, 20(3), članek e12390. CrossRef

Shukla, M., Nespor, M. in Mehler, J. (2007). An interaction between prosody and statistics in the segmentation of fluent speech. Cognitive Psychology, 54(1), 1–32. CrossRef

Siugzdaite, R., Bathelt, J., Holmes, J. in Astle, D. E. (2020). Transdiagnostic brain mapping in developmental disorders. Current Biology, 30(7), 1245–1257. CrossRef

Skinner, B. F. (1963). Behaviorism at fifty. Science, 140(3570), 951–958. CrossRef

Sperber, D. A. N. in Wilson, D. (2002). Pragmatics, modularity and mind-reading. Mind in Language, 17(4), 3–23. CrossRef

Swingley, D. (2005). Statistical clustering and the contents of the infant vocabulary. Cognitive Psychology, 50(1), 86–132. CrossRef

Toro, J. M., Nespor, M., Mehler, J. in Bonatti, L. L. (2008). Finding words and rules in a speech stream: Functional differences between vowels and consonants. Psychological Science, 19(2), 137–144. CrossRef

Toro, J. M. in Trobalón, J. B. (2005). Statistical computations over a speech stream in a rodent. Perception in Psychophysics, 67(5), 867–875. CrossRef

Verguts, T. in Fias, W. (2004). Representation of number in animals and humans: A neural model. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(9), 1493–1504. CrossRef

Werker, J. F. in Tees, R. C. (1999). Influences on infant speech processing: Toward a new synthesis. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 509–535. CrossRef

Yang, C. (2004). Universal grammar, statistics or both? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(10), 451–456. CrossRef

Zerilli, J. (2019a). Neural redundancy and its relation to neural reuse. Philosophy of Science, 86(5), 1191–1201. CrossRef

Zerilli, J. (2019b). Neural reuse and the modularity of mind: Where to next for modularity? Biological Theory, 14(3), 1–20. CrossRef


« Nazaj na Letnik 31 (2022)