Spletna stran Psiholoških obzorij uporablja piškotke za namene avtentikacije uporabnikov po prijavi na spletno stran, morebitno stalno prijavo na željo uporabnika in za namen beleženja števila ogledov posameznih strani Psiholoških obzorij.
Ali se strinjate, da na vaš računalnik (brskalnik) naložimo piškotke za te namene? Svojo odločitev lahko kasneje tudi spremenite na strani Zasebnost.

Želim izvedeti več

Psihološka obzorja :: Horizons of Psychology

Znanstveno-strokovna psihološka revija Društva psihologov Slovenije

Indeksirana v:
Scopus
PsycINFO
Academic OneFile

Smo člani DOAJ in CrossRef

sien
VSEBINA ZA AVTORJE PREDSTAVITEV UREDNIŠTVO POVEZAVE

Iskalnik

Moj račun

Članki z največ ogledi

 

« Nazaj na Letnik 32 (2023)

flag Go to the article page in English / Pojdi na angleško stran članka


Zakaj Platon potrebuje psihologijo? Predlog teoretičnega okvirja za raziskave o kognitivnem transferju učinkov študija klasičnih jezikov

Alexandra Vereeck, Mark Janse, Katja De Herdt, Arnaud Szmalec, Cathy Hauspie in Wouter Duyck

pdf Polno besedilo (pdf)  |  Ogledi: 71  |  flagNapisan v angleščini.  |  Objavljeno: 3. december 2023

pdf https://doi.org/10.20419/2023.32.580  |  Citati: CrossRef (0)

Povzetek: Psihologija je ena od sedmih vozliščnih znanosti, kar po eni strani psihologom nalaga veliko odgovornost, po drugi pa tako za psihologe kot druge znanstvenike prinaša mnogo priložnosti. Navedeno je bila tudi tema 17. Evropskega kongresa psihologije, ki ga je organiziralo Društvo psihologov Slovenije. V članku je podrobno predstavljen primer delovanja psihologije kot vozliščne znanosti. Raziskava svojo vsebino črpa iz na videz nepovezanega področja klasične filologije. Poučevanje latinščine in stare grščine ima v Evropi večstoletno tradicijo, še danes pa se teh jezikov učijo številni dijaki. Hkrati pa je tradicija učenja klasičnih jezikov tudi predmet očitkov, saj so ti v sodobni družbi pogosto zaznani kot nepomembni. Klasični filologi so zato prisiljeni, da branijo sam obstoj svoje stroke. Eden od njihovih argumentov je ta, da ima študij klasičnih jezikov pozitiven učinek na jezikovne in splošne spoznavne zmožnosti dijakov. Ta predpostavka je tesno povezana s splošnim vprašanjem transferja pri učenju, ki že dolgo zaposluje filozofe in psihologe. Edini način, da to tezo podpremo, je da se zatečemo k psihološki metodi. Članek predstavlja prvi celostno elaboriran teoretični okvir za opisovanje kognitivnega učinka učenja klasičnih jezikov in utira pot za metodološko ustrezno in rigorozno raziskovanje na tem področju. Teoretični okvir izhaja iz kognitivnega transferja kot osrednjega konstrukta in ga združuje z vpogledi iz psihološke in druge literature. Tako se vzpostavi plodna interakcija: ne samo, da psihologija prispeva k pomembnosti raziskovanja klasičnih jezikov, ampak s poseganjem na nova področja znanja oplaja tudi kognitivno psihologijo in psiholingvistiko.

Ključne besede: vozliščna znanost, kognitivni transfer, transfer učenja, poučevanje klasičnih jezikov


Citiraj:
Vereeck, A., Janse, M., De Herdt, K., Szmalec, A., Hauspie, C. in Duyck, W. (2023). Why Plato needs psychology. Proposal for a theoretical framework underpinning research on the cognitive transfer effects of studying classical languages. Psihološka obzorja, 121–130. https://doi.org/10.20419/2023.32.580


Seznam literature v članku


Bagheri, M., Hadian, B., & Vaez-Dalili, M. (2019). Effects of the Vaughan method in comparison with the Audiolingual method and the communicative language teaching on Iranian advanced EFL learners' speaking skill. International Journal of Instruction, 12(2), 81–99. CrossRef

Barnett, S., & Ceci, S. (2002). When and where do we apply what we learn? A taxonomy for far transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 128(4), 612–637. CrossRef

Becerra-Bonache, L., & Jiménez-López, D. (2015). A grammatical inference model for measuring language complexity. In I. Rojas, G. Joya, & A. Catala (Eds.), Advances in computational intelligence. 13th International Work-Conference on Artificial Neural Networks, IWANN 2015, Palma de Mallorca, Spain, June 10–12, 2015 (Vol. 1). Springer. CrossRef

Bentz, C., Gutierrez-Vasques, X., Sozinova, O., & Samardžić, T. (2022). Complexity trade-offs and equi-complexity in natural languages: A meta-analysis. Linguistics Vanguard: A Multimodal Journal for the Language Sciences, 14(9), 9–25. CrossRef

Bialystok, E., & Barac, R. (2012). Emerging bilingualism: Dissociating advantages for metalinguistic awareness and executive control. Cognition, 122(1), 67–73. CrossRef

Bialystok, E., Martin, M., & Viswanathan, M. (2005). Bilingualism across the lifespan: The rise and fall of inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 9(1), 103–119. CrossRef

Bracke, E., & Bradshaw, C. (2020). The impact of learning Latin on school pupils. A review of existing data. The Language Learning Journal, 48(2), 226–236. CrossRef

Carlson, S., & Meltzoff, A. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11 (2), 282–298. CrossRef

Castiello, J. (1934). Geistesformung. Beiträge zur experimentellen erforschung der formalen Bildung [Formation of the mind. Contributions about the experimental inquiry into formal discipline]. Dümmler.

Cook, V. (1991). Second language learning and language learning. Edward Arnold.

Dahl, Ö. (2009). Testing the assumption of complexity invariance: the case of Elfdalian and Swedish. In D. Gil, G. Sampson, & P. Trudgill (Eds.), Language complexity as an evolving variable (pp. 50–63). Oxford University Press. CrossRef

De Bruyckere, P., Kirschner, P., & Hulshof, C. (2020). If you learn A, will you be better able to learn B? Understanding transfer of learning. American Educator, 44(1), 30–34.

Dick, A., Garcia, N., Pruden, S., Thompson, W., Hawes, S., Sutherland, M., Riedel, M., Laird, A., & Gonzalez, R. (2019). No evidence for a bilingual executive function advantage in the ABCD study. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(7), 692–701. CrossRef

Fredericks, S. (1974). Vygotsky on language skills. The Classical World, 67, 283–290. CrossRef

Ganschow, L., & Sparks, R. (1995). Effects of direct instruction in Spanish phonology on the native-language skills and foreign-language aptitude of at-risk foreign language learners. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28(2), 107–120. CrossRef

Gil, D., Sampson, G., & Trudgill, P. (Eds.). (2009). Language complexity as an evolving variable. Studies in the evolution of language. Oxford University Press. CrossRef

Gilliland, A. (1922). The effect of the study of Latin on ability to define words. Journal of Educational Psychology, 13 (8), 501–505. CrossRef

Gobet, F., & Sala, G. (2022). Cognitive training: A field in search of a phenomenon. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 20 (10), 1–17. CrossRef

Green, C., Bavelier, D., Kramer, A., Vinogradov, S., Ansorge, U., Ball, K., Bingel, U., Chein, J., Colzato, L., Edwards, J., Facoetti, A., Gazzaley, A., Gathercole, S., Ghisletta, P., Gori, S., Granic, I., Hillman, C., Hommel, B., Jaeggi, S., … Witt, C. (2019). Improving methodological standards in behavioral interventions for cognitive enhancement. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement, 3 (1), 2–29. CrossRef

Hacquebord, H. (2006). Woordkennis als onderdeel van taaldiagnostisch onderzoek [Word knowledge as part of diagnostic research on language]. Levende talen, 7 (1), 15–22.

Harrison, T., Shipstead, Z., Hicks, K., Hambrick, D., Redick, T., & Engle, R. (2013). Working memory training may increase working memory capacity but not fluid intelligence. Psychological Science, 24 (12), 2409–2419. CrossRef

Hille, K., Gust, K., Bitz, U., & Kammer, T. (2011). Associations between music education, intelligence, and spelling ability in elementary school. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 7, 1–6. CrossRef

Housen, A. (2020). Difficulty and complexity of language features and second language instruction. In C. Chapelle (Ed.), The concise encyclopedia of applied linguistics (pp. 389–395). Wiley Blackwell. CrossRef

Housen, A., & Simoens, H. (2016). Introduction: Cognitive perspectives on difficulty and complexity in L2 acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38, 163–175. CrossRef

Hu, M., & Nation, P. (2000). Unknown vocabulary density and reading comprehension. Reading in a Foreign Language, 13(1), 403–430.

Jaeggi, S., Buschkuehl, M., Shah, P., & Jonides, J. (2014). The role of individual differences in cognitive training and transfer. Memory & Cognition, 42(3), 464–480. CrossRef

Janus, M., Lee, Y., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E. (2016). Effects of short-term music and second-language training on executive control. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 144, 84–97. CrossRef

Joseph, J., & Newmeyer, F. (2012). All languages are equally complex. The rise and fall of a consensus. Historiographia Linguistica, 39(2-3), 341–368. CrossRef

Karbach, J., & Kray, J. (2009). How useful is executive control training? Age differences in near and far transfer of task-switching training. Developmental Science, 12(6), 978–990. CrossRef

Larsen-Freeman, D. (2015). Complexity theory. In B. VanPatten, & J. Williams (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition: An introduction (pp. 227–244). Routledge.

Larsen-Freeman, D. (2020). Chaos/complexity theory for second language acquisition/development. In C. Chapelle (Ed.), The concise encyclopedia of applied linguistics (pp. 181–188). Wiley Blackwell. CrossRef

Lloyd, M. & Hunt, S. (2021). Communicative approaches for ancient languages. Bloomsbury Academic. CrossRef

Masciantonio, R. (1977). Tangible benefits of the study of Latin: A review of research. Foreign Language Annals, 10(4), 375–382. CrossRef

Mavrogenes, N. (1977). The effect of elementary latin instruction on language arts performance. The Elementary School Journal, 77(4), 268–73. CrossRef

McWhorter, J. (2001). The world simplest grammars are creole grammars. Linguistic Typology, 5(2-3), 125–166. CrossRef

Murphy, V., Macaro, E., Alba, S., & Cipolla, C. (2015). The influence of learning a second language in primary school on developing first language literacy skills. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36(5), 1133–1153. CrossRef

Paap, K. (2023). The bilingual advantage in executive functioning hypothesis: How the debate provides insight into psychology's replication crisis. Routledge. CrossRef

Pavlenko, A., & Jarvis, S. (2002). Bidirectional transfer. Applied Linguistics, 23(2), 190–214. CrossRef

Peal, E., & Lambert, W. (1962). The relation of bilingualism to intelligence. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 76, 1–23. CrossRef

Pienemann, M., & Lenzing, A. (2015). Processability theory. In B. VanPatten, & J. Williams (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition: An introduction (pp. 159–179). Routledge.

Ritchie, S., & Tucker-Drob, E. (2018). How much does education improve intelligence? A meta-analysis. Psychological Science, 29(8), 1358–1369. CrossRef

Sala, G., Aksayli, N., Tatlidil, S., Tatsumi, T., Gondo, Y., & Gobet, F. (2019). Near and far transfer in cognitive training: A second-order meta-analysis. Collabra: Psychology, 5(1), Article 18. CrossRef

Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2017). Does far transfer exist? Negative evidence from chess, music, and working memory training. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(5), 515–520. CrossRef

Sampson, G. (2009). A linguistic axiom challenged. In D. Gil, G. Sampson, & P. Trudgill (Eds.), Language complexity as an evolving variable (pp. 1–18). Oxford University Press. CrossRef

Sauce, B., & Matzel, L. (2018). The paradox of intelligence: Heritability and malleability coexist in hidden gene-environment interplay. Psychological Bulletin, 144(1), 26–47. CrossRef

Scherer, R., Siddiq, F., & Sánchez Viveros, B. (2019). The cognitive benefits of learning computer programming: A meta-analysis of transfer effects. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111 (5), 764–792. CrossRef

Shipstead, Z., Redick, T., & Engle, R. (2010). Does working memory training generalize? Psychologica Belgica, 50(3–4), 245–276. CrossRef

Spearman, C. (1904). 'General intelligence', objectively determined and measured. The American Journal of Psychology, 15(2), 201–292. CrossRef

Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of man: Their nature and measurement. The Macmillan Company.

Squire, L., & Wixted, J. (2011). The cognitive neuroscience of human memory since H.M. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 34, 259–288. CrossRef

Stellwag, H. (1949). De waarde der klassieke vorming. Een cultuur-historische, paedagogisch-psychologische en didaktische inleiding [The value of classical education. A cultural-historical, pedagogical-psychological and didactical introduction]. Wolters.

Thorndike, E. (1914). The psychology of learning. Teachers College.

Thorndike, E., & Woodworth, R. (1901a). The influence of improvement in one mental function upon the efficiency of other functions. (I). Psychological Review, 8(3), 247–261. CrossRef

Thorndike, E., & Woodworth, R. (1901b). The influence of improvement in one mental function upon the efficiency of other functions. II. The estimation of magnitudes. Psychological Review, 8(4), 384–395. CrossRef

Thorndike, E., & Woodworth, R. (1901c). The influence of improvement in one mental function upon the efficiency of other functions: III. Functions involving attention, observation and discrimination. Psychological Review, 8(6), 553–564. CrossRef

Tiruneh, D., Gu, X., De Cock, M., & Elen, J. (2018). Systematic design of domain-specific instruction on near and far transfer of critical thinking skills. International Journal of Educational Research, 87, 1–11. CrossRef

Ullman, M. (2013). Declarative/procedural model of language. In H. Pashler (Ed.), Encyclopedia of the mind (Vol. 1, pp. 224–226). Sage Publications.

Ullman, M. (2015). The declarative/procedural model. A neurobiologically motivated theory of first and second language. In B. VanPatten, & J. Williams (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition: An introduction (pp. 135–158). Routledge.

Vereeck, A., Bracke, E., De Herdt, K., & Janse, M. (2023). Revered and reviled. An outline of the public debate regarding classical language education. Manuscript in preparation.

Vereeck, A., Janse, M., De Herdt, K., Hauspie, C., Szmalec, A., & Woumans, E. (2023). Does studying Latin make pupils smarter? Presenting the field of classical language impact studies. Manuscript in preparation.

Vereeck, A., Janse, M., De Herdt, K., Szmalec, A., Hauspie, C., & Duyck, W. (2022). A debate in need of data: Report of an empirical study on the cognitive transfer effects of studying classical languages [Conference presentation abstract]. In M. Juriševič (Ed.), Book of Abstracts: 17th European Congress of Psychology. Horizons of Psychology, 31, 59–447.

von Bastian, C., & Oberauer, K. (2014). Effects and mechanisms of working memory training: A review. Psychological Research, 78(6), 803–820. CrossRef

Woumans, E., Surmont, J., Struys, E., & Duyck, W. (2016). The longitudinal effect of bilingual immersion schooling on cognitive control and intelligence. Language Learning, 66(2), 76–91. CrossRef


« Nazaj na Letnik 32 (2023)