« Back to Volume 21 (2012), Issue 3
Pojdi na slovensko stran članka / Go to the article page in Slovene
Brain mechanisms underlying numerical processing
Full text (pdf) | Views: 34 | Written in Slovene. | Published: June 21, 2013
https://doi.org/10.20419/2012.21.370 | Cited By: CrossRef (1)
Abstract: In the paper we present some of the recent findings in the field of numerical processing and brain mechanisms underlying these processes. Quantity estimation and comparison represent the basics of numerical processing. We present this system more in details as this core quantity system is well recognized in infants as well as in some animal species. Core quantity system which is also called mathematical intuition, can be extended by a more complex, arithmetic processing, which is also presented within the article. Understanding the brain mechanisms which underlay numerical processing can help us understand and develop mathematical competence, which represents one of the key cognitive abilities that is acquired through formal schooling.
Keywords: numerical processing, counting, mathematical ability, brain, child development
Cite:
Bregant, T. (2012). Nevrokognitivne osnove numeričnega procesiranja [Brain mechanisms underlying numerical processing]. Psihološka obzorja, 21(3), 69–74. https://doi.org/10.20419/2012.21.370
Reference list
Ansari, D. (2007). Does the parietal cortex distinguish between »10«, »ten«, and ten dots? Neuron, 53, 165–167. CrossRef
Arsalidou, M., & Taylor, M. J. (2011). Is 2+2=4? Meta analyses of brain areas needed for numbers and calculations. Neuroimage, 54, 2382–2393. CrossRef
Blair, K. P., Rosenberg–Lee, M., Tsang, J. M., Schwartz, D. L., & Menon, V. (2012). Beyond natural numbers: Negative number representation in parietal cortex. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(7). CrossRef
Cantlon, J. F., & Brannon, E. M. (2007). How much does number matter to a monkey (Macaca mulatta)? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 33(1), 32–41. CrossRef
Chochon, F., Cohen, L., Moortele, P. F., & Dehaene, S. (1999). Differential contributions of the left and right inferior parietal lobules to number processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 617–630. CrossRef
Cohen Kadosh, R., Bien, N., & Sack, A. T. (2012). Automatic and intentional number processing both rely on intact right parietal cortex: A combined fMRI and neuro–navigated TMS study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(2). CrossRef
Dehaene, S. (2009). Origins of mathematical intuitions: The case of arithmetic. The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience 2009: Annual NewYork Academy of Science, 1156, 232–259. CrossRef
Dehaene, S., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Cohen, L. (2003). Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20(3/4/5/6), 487–506. CrossRef
Dehaene, S., Molko, N., Cohen, L., & Wilson, A. J. (2004). Arithmetic and the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14, 218–224. CrossRef
De Smedt, B., Holloway, I. D., & Ansari, D. (2011). Effects of problem size and arithmetic operation on brain activation during calculation in children with varying levels of arithmetical fluency. NeuroImage, 57, 771–781. CrossRef
Garland, A., Low, J., & Burns, K. C. (2012). Large quantity discrimination by North Island robins (Petroica longipes). Animal cognition, 15, 1129–1140. CrossRef
Grabner, R. H., Reishofer, G., Koschutnig, K., & Ebner, F. (2011). Brain correlates of mathematical competence in processing mathematical representations. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5(130). CrossRef
Houdé, O., Rossi, S., Lubin, A., & Joliot, M. (2010). Mapping numerical processing, reading, and executive functions in the developing brain: An fMRI meta–analysis of 52 studies including 842 children. Developmental Science, 13(6), 876–885. CrossRef
Jordan, K. E., MacLean, E. L., & Brannon, E. (2008). Monkeys match and tally quantities across senses. Cognition, 108(3), 617–625. CrossRef
Leighton, T. G., Richards, S. D., & White, P. R. (2004). Marine mammal signals in bubbly water. V Proceedings of the Institute of Acoustics Symposium on Bio–sonar and Bioacoustics Systems. Institute of Acoustics Symposium on Bio–sonar and Bioacoustics Systems, University of Southampton, Institute of Acoustics.
Nieder, A., & Dehaene, S. (2009). Representation of number in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 32, 185–208. CrossRef
Pepperberg, I. M. (2002). The Alex Studies: Cognitive and communicative abilities of grey parrots. Cambridge, MA, ZDA: Harvard University Press.
Piazza, M. (2010). Neurocognitive start–up tools for symbolic number representations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(12), 542–551. CrossRef
Piazza, M., Pinel, P., Le Bihan, D., & Dehaene, S. (2007). A magnitude code common to numerosities and number symbols in human intraparietal cortex. Neuron, 53, 293–305. CrossRef
Rugani, R., Fontanari, L., Simoni, E., Regolin L., & Vallortigara G. (2009). Arithmetic in newborn chicks. Proceedings of the Royal Society, 276(1666), 2451–2460. CrossRef
Stanescu–Cosson, R., Pinel, P., van de Moortele, P. F., Le Bihan, D., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2000). Understanding dissociation in dyscalculia: A brain imaging study of the impact of number size on the cerebral networks for exact and approximate calculations. Brain, 123, 2240–2255. CrossRef
Thioux, M., Pesenti, M., De Volder, A., & Seron, X. (2002). Category–specific representation and processing of numbers and animal names across semantic tasks: A PET study. NeuroImage, 13(6 suppl. 2/2), S617. CrossRef
Cited By via CrossRef (1)
Mathematical Competence of a Child - Life Success of an Adult
Tina Bregant
Interdisciplinary Description of Complex Systems, 2016
https://doi.org/10.7906/indecs.14.4.3