This site uses cookies for user authentication, optional permanent login and monitoring the number of page views (Google Analytics).
Do you agree with cookies being used in accordance with our Privacy policy? You can change your decision regarding the use of cookies on the Privacy page.

I want to know more

Horizons of Psychology :: Psihološka obzorja

Scientific and Professional Psychological Journal of the Slovenian Psychologists' Association

Indexed in:
Scopus
PsycINFO
Academic OneFile

Member of DOAJ and CrossRef

sien
CONTENTS FOR AUTHORS ABOUT EDITORIAL BOARD LINKS

Search

My Account

Most viewed articles

 

« Back to Volume 25 (2016)

flag Pojdi na slovensko stran članka / Go to the article page in Slovene


Relationship between children's theory of mind and metalinguistic competence in early childhood

Kaja Hacin

pdf Full text (pdf)  |  Views: 253  |  flagWritten in English.  |  Published: September 8, 2016

pdf https://doi.org/10.20419/2016.25.448  |  Cited By: CrossRef (1)

Abstract: The aim of this study was to examine the relations between the theory of mind and metalinguistic competences in early childhood. The sample included 45 children aged from 4;6 to 6 years from 5 different Slovene preschools. The False belief task and the Pictures sequencing task were used to assess children's theory of mind. Children's metalinguistic competences were assessed with the Little glove storytelling test and the Scales of general language development. Children's parents also completed a questionnaire, designed to obtain information about children's play. The results showed significant correlations between children's scores on the majority of tasks. A significant relationship was found between children's theory of mind, their metalinguistic awareness and their storytelling competence, especially the content structure of their story. This association might reflect similar demands for children's understanding of representations that is necessary for the theory of mind and metalinguistic competences. Furthermore, there was no significant correlation between the scores on the Picture sequencing task and children's false belief understanding, but there was a significant correlation between these scores and children's scores on the tasks of metalinguistic competences, which could be an indicator of a relation between child's sequencing of pictures into a complete story and their metalinguistic competences. Results did not show significant differences between children's theory of mind or their metalinguistic awareness based on the type of their most frequent play. Thus, the study provides a complex insight into relations between different aspects of children's theory of mind and metalinguistic competences; and as such highlights the strong relationship between both constructs.

Keywords: theory of mind, metalinguistic competence, metalinguistic awareness, storytelling, early childhood


Cite:
Hacin, K. (2016). Relationship between children's theory of mind and metalinguistic competence in early childhood. Psihološka obzorja, 25, 72–83. https://doi.org/10.20419/2016.25.448


Reference list


Antonietti, A., Liverta-Sempio, O., Marchetti, A., & Astington, J. W. (2006). Mental language and understanding of epistemic and emotional mental states. In A. Antonietti, O. Liverta-Sempio and A. Marchetti (Eds.), Theory of mind and language in developmental contexts, (pp. 1–30). New York, NY, USA: Springer Science+Business Media. CrossRef

Applebee, A. N. (1978). The child's concept of story. Chicago, IL, USA: University of Chicago Press.

Astington, J. W. (2001). The future of theory-of-mind research: Understanding motivational states, the role of language, and real-world consequences. Child development, 72(3), 685–687. CrossRef

Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35(5), 1311–1320. CrossRef

Baron Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a »theory of mind«? Cognition, 21, 37–46. CrossRef

Baron Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1986). Mechanical, behavioural and intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 113–125. CrossRef

Bloom, P., & German, T. P. (2000). Two reasons to abandon the false-belief task as a test of theory of mind. Cognition, 77, 25–31. CrossRef

Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (1999). A nonverbal false belief task: The performance of children and great apes. Child Development, 70(2), 381–395. CrossRef

Charman, T., & Shmueli Goetz, Y. (1998). The relationship between theory of mind, language, and narrative discourse: An experimental study. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/Current Psychology of Cognition, 17(2), 245–271.

Cutting, A. L., & Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind, emotional understanding, language, and family background: Individual differences and interrelations. Child development, 70(4), 853–865. CrossRef

de Villiers, J. (2007). The interface of language and theory of mind. Lingua, 117, 1858–1878. CrossRef

de Villiers, J., & de Villiers, P. (1972). Early judgements of semantic and syntactic acceptability by children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 1, 299–310. CrossRef

Doherty, M. (2000). Children's understanding of homonymy: Metalinguistic awareness and FB. Journal of Child Language, 27(2), 367–392. CrossRef

Doherty, M., & Perner, J. (1998). Metalinguistic awareness and theory of mind: Just two words for the same thing? Cognitive Development, 13, 279–305. CrossRef

Farrar, M. J., & Ashwell, S. (2012). Phonological awareness, executive functioning, and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 27, 77–89. CrossRef

Farrar, M. J., Ashwell, S., & Maag, L. (2005). The emergence of phonological awareness: Connections to language and theory of mind development. First language, 25(2), 157–172. CrossRef

Fekonja, U. (2009). Razvoj otroške igre [Child play development]. In L. Marjanovič Umek & M. Zupančič (Eds.), Razvojna psihologija [Developmental psychology] (pp. 382–393). Ljubljana, Slovenia: Znanstveni inštitut Filozofske fakultete.

Gamannossi, B. A., & Pinto, G. (2014). Theory of mind and language of mind in narratives: Developmental trends from kindergarten to primary school. First language, 34(3), 262–272. CrossRef

Gopnik, A., & Astington, J. W. (1988). Children's understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction. Child development, 59, 26–37. CrossRef

Hughes, C., & Dunn, J. (1997). "Pretend you didn't know": Preschoolers' talk about mental states in pretend play. Cognitive Development, 12, 477–499. CrossRef

Hughes, C., & Dunn, J. (1998). Understanding mind and emotion: Longitudinal associations with mental-state talk between young friends. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1026–1037. CrossRef

Jenkins, J. M., & Astington, J. W. (1996). Cognitive factors and family structure associated with theory of mind development in young children. Developmental Psychology, 32, 70–78. CrossRef

Jenkins, J. M., & Astington, J. W. (2000). Theory of mind and social behaviour: Causal models tested in a longitudinal study. Merrill-Palmer Quaterly, 46(2), 203–220.

Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of »Theory of mind«. Psychological Review, 94(4), 412–426. CrossRef

Marjanovič Umek, L. (2009). Spoznavni razvoj v zgodnjem otroštvu [Cognitive development in early childhood]. In L. Marjanovič Umek & M. Zupančič (Eds.), Razvojna psihologija [Developmental psychology] (pp. 291–314). Ljubljana, Slovenia: Znanstveni inštitut Filozofske fakultete.

Marjanovič Umek, L., & Fekonja, U. (2009). Razvoj govora v zgodnjem otroštvu [Speech development in early childhood]. In L. Marjanovič Umek & M. Zupančič (Eds.), Razvojna psihologija [Developmental psychology] (pp. 315–333). Ljubljana, Slovenia: Znanstveni inštitut Filozofske fakultete.

Marjanovič Umek, L., & Fekonja Peklaj, U. (2010). The role of symbolic play in early literacy development. Journal of Communications Research, 1(4), 291–308.

Marjanovič Umek, L., Fekonja, U., Podlesek, A., Kranjc, S, & Bajc, K. (2007). Lestvice splošnega govornega razvoja – LJ (LSGR – LJ): Priročnik [Scales of general language development – LJ: Manual]. Ljubljana, Slovenia: Center za psihodiagnostična sredstva.

Marjanovič Umek, L., Fekonja Peklaj, U., Sočan, G., & Komidar, L. (2011). Pripovedovanje zgodbe: Preizkus pripovedovanja zgodbe: Rokavička: Preizkus pripovedovanja zgodbe: Žabji kralj: Priročnik [Storytelling: Storytelling test: The Little glove: Storytelling test: The Frog king: Manual]. Ljubljana, Slovenia: Center za psihodiagnostična sredstva.

Marjanovič Umek, L., & Lešnik Musek, P. (1999). Simbolna igra: Kaj jo določa in kako igra določa otrokov razvoj [Symbolic play: What determines it and how the play determines the child's development]. Psihološka obzorja, 8(2-3), 3–58.

Miller, C. A. (2006). Developmental relationships between language and theory of mind. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 15, 142–154. CrossRef

Milligan, K., Astington J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child development, 78(2), 622–646. CrossRef

Moore, C., Pure, K., & Furrow, D. (1990). Children's understanding of the modal expression of speaker certainty and uncertainty and its relation to the development of a representational theory of mind. Child development, 61, 722–730. CrossRef

Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA, USA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.

Perner, J., Baker, S., & Hutton, D. (1994). Prelief: The conceptual origins of belief and pretence. In C. Lewis and P. Mitchell (Eds.), Children's early understanding of mind: Origins and development (pp. 261–286). Hove, United Kingdom: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Perner, J., Stummer, S., Sprung, M., & Doherty, M. (2002). Theory of mind finds its Piagetian perspective: Why alternative naming comes with understanding belief. Cognitive Development, 17, 1451–1472. CrossRef

Slade, L., & Ruffman, T. (2005). How language does (and does not) relate to theory of mind: A longitudinal study of syntax, semantics, working memory and false belief. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 117–141. CrossRef

Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory of mind development: The truth about false-belief. Child Development, 72, 655–684. CrossRef

Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128. CrossRef

Youngblade, L. M., & Dunn, J. (1995). Individual differences in young children's pretend play with mother and siblings: Links to relationship and understanding of other people's feelings and beliefs. Child Development, 66, 1472–1492. CrossRef

Zalla, T., Bouchilloux, N., Labruyere, N., Georgieff, N., Bougerol, T., & Franck, N. (2006). Impairment in event sequencing in disorganised and non-disorganised patients with schizophrenia. Brain Research Bulletin, 68, 195–202. CrossRef


Cited By via CrossRef (1)

Analisis Picture Storybook dalam Meningkatkan Kemampuan Theory of Mind Anak Usia Dini
       Elsy Gusmayanti, Yulia Ayriza
       Jurnal Obsesi : Jurnal Pendidikan Anak Usia Dini, 2023
       https://doi.org/10.31004/obsesi.v7i1.2726


« Back to Volume 25 (2016)