Pojdi na slovensko stran članka / Go to the article page in Slovene
Effectiveness of cognitive training with the "Misleca" exercise book in middle childhood
Zala Strojnik & Anja Podlesek
Full text (pdf) | Views: 104 | Written in Slovene. | Published: May 7, 2021
https://doi.org/10.20419/2021.30.533 | Cited By: CrossRef (0)
Abstract: Studies on the effectiveness of cognitive training do not show consistent results. While some have shown positive effects of the training on fluid intelligence and other cognitive abilities, others have not. Studies of the effectiveness of cognitive training in normative middle childhood children and studies on multicomponent cognitive training are rare. The aim of our study was to investigate the effect of cognitive training in the form of regular solving of mental tasks from the “Misleca” manual on cognitive abilities in middle childhood. Thirty-eight children with a mean age of 8 years 5 months (SD = 3 months) were randomly divided into two halves, forming an experimental and a control group. After the pre-tests to measure fluid intelligence, verbal fluency, working memory, switching ability, inhibition and visuo-spatial planning, the experimental group participated in the training, which was conducted three times a week over a period of six weeks. After each session the children reported on their motivation for the training. The control group was passive. After six weeks, cognitive tests were carried out again. The results showed a positive effect of the training on the Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices score as a measure of fluid intelligence, while no effect on other cognitive abilities was observed. The results also showed that the children found the training interesting and that the majority were motivated to solve the tasks.
Keywords: cognitive training, fluid intelligence, executive functions, children
Cite:
Strojnik, Z., & Podlesek, A. (2021). Učinkovitost kognitivnega treninga z zbirko Misleca pri otrocih v srednjem otroštvu [Effectiveness of cognitive training with the "Misleca" exercise book in middle childhood]. Psihološka obzorja, 30, 88–100. https://doi.org/10.20419/2021.30.533
Reference list
Anderson, P., & Reidy, N. (2012). Assessing executive function in preschoolers. Neuropsychology Review, 22(4), 345–360. CrossRef
Barkl, S., Porter, A., & Ginns, P. (2012). Cognitive training for children: Effects on inductive reasoning, deductive reasoning, and mathematics achievement in an Australian school setting. Psychology in the Schools, 49(9), 828–842. CrossRef
Beck, D. M., Schaefer, C., Pang, K., & Carlson, S. M. (2011). Executive function in preschool children: Test-retest reliability. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12(2), 169–193. CrossRef
Binder, J. C., Zöllig, J., Eschen, A., Mérillat, S., Röcke, C., Schoch, S. F., Jäncke, L., & Martin, M. (2015). Multi-domain training in healthy old age: Hotel Plastisse as an iPad-based serious game to systematically compare multi-domain and single-domain training. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 7, 137–158. CrossRef
Bucik, N., Bucik, G., & Bucik, V. (2013). Misleca: Miselne naloge za mlade bistroume [Misleca collection of mental tasks]. Mladinska knjiga.
Buitenweg, J. I. V., van de Ven, R. M., Prinssen, S., Murre, J. M. J., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2017). Cognitive flexibility training: A large-scale multimodal adaptive active-control intervention study in healthy older adults. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 529–542. CrossRef
Burge, W. K., Ross, L. A., Amtho, F. R., Mitchell, W. G., Zotov, A., & Visscher, K. M. (2013). Processing speed training increases the efficiency of attentional resource allocation in young adults. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 684–690. CrossRef
Cattell, R. B. (1963). Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54(1), 1–22. CrossRef
Chapman, S. S., Ewing, C. B., & Mozzoni, M. P. (2005). Precision teaching and fluency training across cognitive, physical, and academic tasks in children with traumatic brain injury: A multiple baseline study. Behavioral Interventions, 20(1), 37–49. CrossRef
Cheng, Y., Wu, W., Feng, W., Wang, J., Chen, Y., Shen, Y., Li, Q., Zhang, X., & Li, C. (2012). The effects of multi-domain versus single-domain cognitive training in non-demented older people: A randomized controlled trial. BMC Medicine, 10(1). CrossRef
Chooi, W. T., & Thompson, L. A. (2012). Working memory training does not improve intelligence in healthy young adults. Intelligence, 40(6), 531–542. CrossRef
de Vries, M., Prins, P. J., Schmand, B. A., & Geurts, H. M. (2014). Working memory and cognitive flexibility-training for children with an autism spectrum disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 56(5), 566–576. CrossRef
Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(3), 309–331. CrossRef
Fernández, A., & Marcopulos, B. A. (2008). A comparison of normative data for the Trail Making Test from several countries: Equivalence of norms and considerations for interpretation. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 49(3), 239–246. CrossRef
Gottfredson, L. S. (1997). Mainstream science on intelligence: An editorial with 52 signatories, history, and bibliography. Intelligence, 24(1), 13–23. CrossRef
Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2008). Exercising your brain: A review of human brain plasticity and training-induced learning. Psychology and Aging, 23(4), 692–701. CrossRef
Hager, W., & Hasselhorn, M. (1998). The effectiveness of the cognitive training for children from a differential perspective: A meta- evaluation. Learning and Instruction, 8(5), 411–438. CrossRef
Harrison, T. L., Shipstead, Z., Hicks, K. L., Hambrick, D. Z.,. Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2013). Working memory training may increase working memory capacity but not fluid intelligence. Psychological Science, 24(12), 2409–2419. CrossRef
Heugten, C. M., Ponds, R. W. H. M., & Kessels, R. P. C. (2016). Brain training: Hype or hope?Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 26(5–6), 639–644. CrossRef
Hussey, E. K., Teubner-Rhodes, S. E., Velnoskey, K., Harbison, J. I., Mishler, A.,, & Novick, J. M. (2017). Memory and language improvements following cognitive control training. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 43(1), 23–58. CrossRef
Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(19), 6829–6833. CrossRef
Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Shah, P. (2011). Short- and long-term benefits of cognitive training. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(25), 10081–10086. CrossRef
Jaeggi, M. S., Studer-Luethi, B., Buschkuehl, M., Su, Y., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. (2010). The relationship between n-back performance and matrix reasoning - Implications for training and transfer. Intelligence, 38(6), 625–635. CrossRef
Johann, J. E., & Karbach, J. (2019). Effects of game‐based and standard executive control training on cognitive and academic abilities in elementary school children. Developmental Science, 23(2), članek e12866. CrossRef
Klauer, K. J. (1989). Denktraining für Kinder I [Cognitivne training for children I]. Hogrefe.
Krečič, K. (2013). Razvoj Testa semantične fluentnosti [Development of Semantic Fluency Test] [Neobjavljeno diplomsko delo]. Univerza v Ljubljani, Filozofska fakulteta.
Kuther, T. L. (2019). Lifespan development: Lives in context (2. izd.). SAGE Publications.
Lee, D. Kwak, S., & Jeanyung, C. (2019). Parallel changes in cognitive function and gray matter volume after multi-component training of cognitive control (MTCC) in adolescents. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 13, 246–258. CrossRef
Lenhard, A., & Lenhard, W. (2011). Computerbasierte Intelligenzförderung mit den "Denkspielen mit Elfe und Mathis" – Vorstellung und Evaluation eines Computerprogramms für Vor-und Grundschüler[Computer based intelligence training with „Thinking Games with Elfe und Mathis“ – Presentation and evaluation of a computer program for preschool and elementary school]. Empirische Sonderpädagogik, 3(2), 105–120. CrossRef
Mair, P., & Wilcox, R. R. (2019). Robust statistical methods in R using the WRS2 package. Behavior Research Methods, 52(2), 464–488. CrossRef
McGrew, K. S. (2009). CHC theory and the human cognitive abilities project: Standing on the shoulders of the giants of psychometric intelligence research. Intelligence, 37(1), 1–10. CrossRef
Muniz, M., Gomes, C. M. A., & Pasian, S. R. (2016). Factor structure of Raven's coloured progressive matrices. Psico-USF, 21(2), 259–272. CrossRef
Nouchi, R., Saito, T., Nouchi, H., & Kawashima, R. (2016). Small acute benefits of 4 weeks processing speed training games on processing speed and inhibition performance and depressive mood in the healthy elderly people: Evidence from a randomized control trial. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 8, 302–313. CrossRef
Nousia, A., Siokas, V., Aretouli, E., Messinis, L., Aloizou, A., Martzoukou, M., Karala, M., Koumpoulis, C., Nasios, G.,, & Dardiotis, E. (2018). Beneficial effect of multidomain cognitive training on the neuropsychological performance of patients with early-stage Alzheimer's disease. Neural Plasticity, 2018, članek 2845176. CrossRef
Phye, G. D., & Johnson, S. A. (2009). Cognitive training: Improving problem-solving transfer skills of learning disabled students. Educational and Child Psychology, 26(3), 59–70.
Primi, R., Ferrao, M. E., & Almeida, L. S. (2010). Fluid intelligence as a predictor of learning: A longitudinal multilevel approach applied to math. Learning and Individual Differences, 20(5), 446–451. CrossRef
Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. C. (1990). Manual for Raven's Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales - Section 2: Coloured Progressive Matrices. Oxford Psychologists Press.
Raven, J. C., Styles, I., & Raven, M. (1998). Barvne progresivne matrice [Raven's Progressive Matrics] [Merski pripomoček]. Center za psihodiagnostična sredstva.
Redick, T. S., Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T. L., Hicks, K. L., Fried, D. E., Hambrick, D .Z., Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2013). No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: A randomized, placebo-controlled study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(2), 359–379. CrossRef
Rudebeck, S. R., Bor, D., Ormond, A., O'Reilly, J. X., & Lee, A. C. H. (2012). A potential spatial working memory training task to improve both episodic memory and fluid intelligence. PloS ONE, 11(7), članek e50431. CrossRef
Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2017). Working memory training in typically developing children: A meta-analysis of the available evidence. Developmental Psychology, 53(4), 671–685. CrossRef
Shao, Z. Janse, E., Visser, K., & Meyer, A. S. (2014). What do verbal fluency tasks measure? Predictors of verbal fluency performance in older adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 722–731. CrossRef
Shelton, J. T, Elliott, E. M., Matthews, R. A., Hill, B. D., & Gouvier, W. D. (2010). The relationships of working memory, secondary memory, and general fluid intelligence: Working memory is special. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(3), 813–820. CrossRef
Simons, D. J., Boot, W. R., Charness, N., Gathercole, S. E., Chabris, C. F., Hambrick, D. Z., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. L. (2016). Do "brain-training" programs work? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 17(3), 103–186. CrossRef
Siquara, G. M., dos Santos Lima, C., & Abreu, N. (2018). Working memory and intelligence quotient: Which best predicts on school achievement? Psico, 49(4), 365–374. CrossRef
Slana, A., Pečjak, S., & Repovš, G. (2017). Testna baterija za merjenje izvršilnih funkcij [Test battery for measuring executive functions] [Neobjavljen merski pripomoček]. Univerza v Ljubljani, Filozofska fakulteta, Oddelek za psihologijo.
Slate, S. E., Meyer, T. L., Burns, W. J., & Montgomery, D. D. (1998). Computerized cognitive training for severely emotionally disturbed children with ADHD. Behavior Modification, 22(3), 415–437. CrossRef
Smith, M., Jones, M. P., Dotson, M. M.,, & Wolinsky, F. D. (2018). Speed-of-processing training in assisted and independent living: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 66(8), 1538–1545. CrossRef
Soveri, A., Antfolk, J., Karlsson, L., Salo, B., & Laine, M. (2017). Working memory training revisited: A multi-level meta-analysis of n-back training studies. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(4), 1077–1096. CrossRef
Steiner, N. J., Frenette, E. C., Rene, K. M., Brennan R. T., & Perrin, E. C. (2014). In-school neurofeedback training for ADHD: Sustained improvements from a randomized control trial. Pediatrics, 133(3), 483–492. CrossRef
Sternberg, R. J. (2008). Increasing fluid intelligence is possible after all. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(19), 6791–6792. CrossRef
Tacol, L. (2018). Povezave med telesnimi merami, gibalnimi sposobnostmi in izvršilnimi funkacijami otroštvu [Correlations between body mass index, motor skills and executive functions in childhood] [Magistrsko delo, Univerza v Ljubljani, Pedagoška fakulteta]. PeFprints. http:// http://pefprints.pef.uni-lj.si/5612/
Thorell, L. B., Veleiro, A., Siu, A. F. Y., & Mohammadi, H. (2013). Examining the relation between ratings of executive functioning and academic achievement: Findings from a cross-cultural study. Child Neuropsychology, 19(6), 630–638. CrossRef
Trail Making Test (TMT) parts A & B. (b. d.). University of South Dakota Internet Psychology Lab. http://apps.usd.edu/coglab/schieber/psyc423/pdf/IowaTrailMaking.pdf
Tsolaki, M., Poptsi, E., Aggogiatou, C., Markou, N., Zafeiropoulos, S., & Kounti, F. (2017). Computer-based cognitive training versus paper and pencil training: Which is more effective? A randomized controlled trial in people with mild cognitive impairment. JSM Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementia, 4(1), 1032–1038.
Valdes, E. G., O'Connorb, M. L., Uc, E. Y., Hauser, R. A., Andel, R. in. Edwards, J. D. (2017). Use, maintenance and dose effects of cognitive speed of processing training in Parkinson's disease. International Journal of Neuroscience, 127(10), 841–848. CrossRef
Wass, S. V. (2015). Applying cognitive training to target executive functions during early development. Child Neuropsychology, 21(2), 150–166. CrossRef
Wilcox, R. R. (2010). Fundamentals of modern statistical methods: Substantially improving power and accuracy (2. izd.). Springer.
Wilcox, R. R. (2012). Introduction to robust estimation and hypothesis testing (3. izd.). Academic Press. CrossRef
Wild-Wall, N., Falkenstein, M., & Gajewski, P. D. (2012). Neural correlates of changes in a visual search task due to cognitive training in seniors. Neural Plasticity, 2012, članek 529057. CrossRef
Woods, D. L., Kishiyama, M. M., Yund, E. W., Herron, T. J., Edwards, B., Poliva, O. Hink, R. F., & Reed, B. (2011). Improving digit span assessment of short-term verbal memory. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33(1), 101–111. CrossRef
Wujcik, R., Nowak, M., & Nęcka, E. (2017). Enhancement of executive control through short-term cognitive training: Far-transfer effects on general fluid intelligence. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 48(1), 72–78. CrossRef
Zhao, X., Chen, L., & Maes, J. H. R. (2016). Training and transfer effects of response inhibition training in children and adults. Developmental Science, 21(1), članek e12511. CrossRef
Zupančič, M. (2009). Predmet in zgodovina razvojne psihologije [The subject and history of developmental psychology]. V L. Marjanovič Umek in M. Zupančič (ur.), Razvojna psihologija [Developmental psychology] (str. 6-27). Rokus Klett.