This site uses cookies for user authentication, optional permanent login and monitoring the number of page views (Google Analytics).
Do you agree with cookies being used in accordance with our Privacy policy? You can change your decision regarding the use of cookies on the Privacy page.

I want to know more

Horizons of Psychology :: Psihološka obzorja

Scientific and Professional Psychological Journal of the Slovenian Psychologists' Association

Indexed in:
Scopus
PsycINFO
Academic OneFile

Member of DOAJ and CrossRef

sien
CONTENTS FOR AUTHORS ABOUT EDITORIAL BOARD LINKS

Search

My Account

Most viewed articles

 

« Back to Volume 30 (2021)

flag Pojdi na slovensko stran članka / Go to the article page in Slovene


Computerised cognitive training in pupils with mild intellectual disability

Nastasija Mahne, Karin Bakračevič & Vojko Kavčič

pdf Full text (pdf)  |  Views: 102  |  flagWritten in Slovene.  |  Published: May 7, 2021

pdf https://doi.org/10.20419/2021.30.534  |  Cited By: CrossRef (0)

Abstract: A lot of studies investigated near and far transfer effects of cognitive training (especially working memory training), but only few were carried out on children with mild intellectual disability. For this reason, we wanted to study the effects of computerised cognitive training (using virtual labyrinth) on cognitive functions of children with mild intellectual disability. We were particularly interested in its effects on executive functions and the related cognitive functions: shifting, verbal fluency, working memory, long-term memory and processing speed. Twenty-one fourth to ninth graders aged 10 to 19 participated, 11 in the experimental and 10 in the control group. The experimental group participated in 12 training sessions, each lasting 35 minutes. Intervention took place in groups, twice a week. The control group was passive and was not involved in any activities. Before and after trainings both groups were tested with selected neuropsychological tests and the CogState computer battery. The results showed that in the second wave, the experimental group considerably improved its results on several measurements of cognitive functions. Compared to the control group, experimental group showed significantly higher total gains on the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT; total score and test IV score) and the Trail Making Test, Part A. The training therefore had the largest effect on the speed of processing visual information and verbal memory.

Keywords: computerised cognitive training, children with mild intellectual disability, executive functions, memory, processing speed


Cite:
Mahne, N., Bakračevič, K., & Kavčič, V. (2021). Računalniški kognitivni trening pri otrocih in mladostnikih z lažjo motnjo v duševnem razvoju [Computerised cognitive training in pupils with mild intellectual disability]. Psihološka obzorja, 30, 79–87. https://doi.org/10.20419/2021.30.534


Reference list


Bangirana, P., Sikorskii, A., Giordani, B., Nakasujja, N., & Boivin, M. J. (2015). Validation of the CogState battery for rapid neurocognitive assessment in Ugandan school age children. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 9(38), 1–7. CrossRef

Barič, M. (2009). Struktura modela svetovanja za ranljivo skupino oseb s posebnimi potrebami - Osebe z lažjo motnjo v duševnem razvoju [The structure of the model of counseling for the vulnerable group of individuals with special needs - Individuals with mild cognitive disability] (poročilo operacije Razvoj modelov svetovanja za ranljive skupine). Ministrstvo RS za šolstvo in šport. http://mizs.arhiv-spletisc.gov.si/fileadmin/mizs.gov.si/pageuploads/podrocje/odrasli/Gradiva_ESS/CVZU/razvoj_modelov/CVZU_7MODELI_Struktura_lazja.pdf

Bergman Nutley, S., Soderqvist, S., Bryde, S., Thorell, L. B., Humphreys, K., & Klingberg,T. (2011). Gains in fluid intelligence after training non-verbal reasoning in 4-year-old children: A controlled, randomized study. Developmental Science, 14(3), 591–601. CrossRef

Brown, T. E. (2005). Attention deficit disorder: The unfocused mind in children and adults. Yale University Press.

Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A., & Shell, P. (1990). What one intelligence test measures: A theoretical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices Test. Psychological Review, 97(3), 404–431. CrossRef

Charter, R. A., Adkins, T. G., Alekoumbides, A., & Seacat, G. F. (1987). Reliability of the WAIS, WMS, and Reitan Battery: Raw scores and standardized scores corrected for age and education. International Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 9, 28–32.

Cogstate. (2018). Cogstate Research Battery. https://www.cogstate.com/academic-research/

Crowe, S. F. (1998). The differential contribution of mental tracking, cognitive flexibility, visual search, and motor speed to performance on parts A and B of the Trail Making Test. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 54(5), 585–591. CrossRef

Danielsson, H., Zottarel, V., Palmqvist, L., & Lanfranchi, S. (2015). The effectiveness of working memory training with individuals with intellectual disabilities - A meta-analytic review. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–10. CrossRef

Dawson, P., & Guare, R. (2009). Executive Skills: The Hidden Curriculum. Principal Leadership, 9(7), 10–14.

Delavarian, M., Bokharaeian, B., Towhidkhah, F., & Gharibzadeh, S. (2015). Computer-based working memory training in children with mild intellectual disability. Early Child Development and Care, 185(1), 66–74. CrossRef

Franzen, M. D., Paul, D., & Iverson, G. L. (1996). Reliability of alternate forms of the Trail Making Test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 10, 125–129. CrossRef

Hammers, D., Spurgeon, E., Ryan, K., Persad, C., Barbas, N., Heidebrink, J., Darby, D., & Giordani, B. (2012).Validity of a brief computerized cognitive screening test in dementia. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 25(2), 89–99. CrossRef

Holmes, J., Gathercole, S. E., & Dunning, D. L. (2009). Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children. Developmental Science, 12(4), F9–F15. CrossRef

Klingberg, T. (2010). Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(7), 317–324. CrossRef

Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P. J., Johnson, M., Gustafsson, P., Dahlström, K., Gillberg, C. G., Forssberg, H., & Westerberg, H. (2005). Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD: A randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(2), 177–186. CrossRef

Lah, S. (2017). Kognitivni računalniški trening kot dopolnitev dodatne strokovne pomoči in njegova učinkovitost pri učencih s posebnimi potrebami [Cognitive computer training as a completion of additional professional support and its effectiveness for pupils with special needs] [Magistrsko delo, Univerza v Mariboru, Filozofska fakulteta].Digitalna knjižnica Univerze v Mariboru. https://dk.um.si/Dokument.php?id=120606

Lezak, M. (2012). Neuropsychological assessment. Oxford University Press.

Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment (4. izd.) . Oxford University Press.

Marinč, D., Burnik, F., Vališer, A., Barborič, K., Potočnik Dajčman, N., & Dretnik, F. (2015). Otroci z motnjami v duševnem razvoju. V N. Vovk-Ornik (ur.), Kriteriji za opredelitev vrste in stopnje primanjkljajev, ovir oz. motenj otrok s posebnimi potrebami (str. 6–7). Zavod RS za šolstvo.

Marusic, U., Giordani, B., Moffat, S., Petrič, M., Dolenc, P., Pišot, R., & Kavcic, V. (2016). Computerized cognitive training during physical inactivity improves executive functioning in older adults. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 25(1), 1–21. CrossRef

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Rettinger, D. A., Shah, P., & Hegarty, M. (2001). How are visuospatial working memory, executive functioning, and spatial abilities related? A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(4), 621–640. CrossRef

Moalli, E. (2006). Conoscenze sulla memoria e prestazioni di memoria in individuicon sindrome di Down e con sviluppo tipico: Effetti di training di breve durata[Knowledge about memory and memory performance in individuals with Down syndrome and with typical development: Effects of short duration training] [Neobjavljena doktorska disertacija]. Univerza v Padovi.

Moalli, E., Rota Negroni, S., & Vianello, R. (2004). Conoscenze sulla memoria eprestazioni di memoria in ragazzi con sindrome di Down: Effetti di due diversitraining di breve durata [Knowledge about memory and memory performance in boys with Down syndrome: effects of two different short-term trainings]. Giornale Italiano delle Disabilitá, 3, 23–40.

Peña-Casanova, J., Quiñones-Úbeda, S., Gramunt-Fombuena, N., Quintana-Aparicio, M., Aguilar, M., Badenes, D., Cerulla, N., Molinuevo, J. L., Ruiz, E., Robles, A., Sagrario Barquero, M., Antunez, C., Martinez-Parra, C., Frank-Garcia, A., Fernandez, M., Alfonso, V., Sol, J. M., & Blesa, R. (2009). Spanish Multicenter Normative Studies (NEURONORMA Project): Norms for Verbal Fluency Tests. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24(4), 395–411. CrossRef

Ramier, A. M., & Hécaen, H. (1970). Role respectif des atteintes frontales et de la late ́ralisation le ́sionnelle dans les de ́ficits de la fluence verbale [Respective role of frontal impairment and lesion lateralization in verbal fluency deficits]. Revue Neurologique, 132, 17–22.

Salthouse, T. A., Fristoe, N., McGuthry, K. E., & Hambrick, D. Z. (1998). Relation of task switching to speed, age, and fluid intelligence. Psychology and Aging, 13(3), 445–461. CrossRef

Söderqvist, S., Nutley, S. B., Ottersen, J., Grill, K. M., & Klingberg, T. (2012). Computerized training of non-verbal reasoning and working memory in children with intellectual disability. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 200–207. CrossRef

Teleb, A., Mohamed, W., & Elbert, T. (2016). Does enhancing visual perception in mild intellectually disabled children transfer to other skills? European Proceedings of Social and Behavioural Sciences (EpSBS): ICEEPSY 2016, 16, članek 41, 391–402. CrossRef

Thorell, L. B., Lindqvist, S., Berman Nutley, S., Bohlin, G., & Klingberg, T. (2009). Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Developmental Science, 12(1), 106–113. CrossRef

Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G. (1997). Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: Evidence from younger and older healthy adults. Neuropsychology, 11(1), 138–146. CrossRef

Vakil, E., & Blachstein, H. (1993). Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test: Structure analysis. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 49(6), 883–890. CrossRef

Van der Molen, M. J., Van Luit, J. E. H., Van der Molen, M. W., Klugkist, I., & Jongmans, M. J. (2010). Effectiveness of a computerised working memory training in adolescents with mild to borderline intellectual disabilities. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 54(5), 433–447. CrossRef

van 't Hooft, I., Andersson, K., Bergman, B., Sejersen, T., von Wendt, L., & Bartfai, A. (2007). Sustained favorable effects of cognitive training in children with acquired brain injuries. NeuroRehabilitation, 22(2), 109–16. CrossRef


« Back to Volume 30 (2021)