Go to the article page in English / Pojdi na angleško stran članka
Kodiranje informacije o vrstnem redu v delovnem spominu: pregled vedenjskih raziskav in kognitivnih mehanizmov
Barbara Dolenc in Grega Repovš
Polno besedilo (pdf) | Ogledi: 214 | Napisan v slovenščini. | Objavljeno: 8. junij 2014
https://doi.org/10.20419/2014.23.391 | Citati: CrossRef (0)
Povzetek: Za kognitivne naloge višjega reda so izredno pomembni izvršilni procesi, med katere spada tudi kodiranje časovnega vrstnega reda informacij. Pomembno vprašanje je, kako se vrstni red kodira ter kateri kognitivni in živčni procesi tvorijo njegovo podlago. Namen pričujočega članka je povzeti izsledke raziskav, ki preverjajo, ali spomina za vrstni red in prepoznavo dražljajev temeljita na ločenih procesih, ter pregledati nekaj osnovnih kognitivnih mehanizmov, ki razlagajo kodiranje vrstnega reda informacij v delovnem spominu. Številne študije kažejo, da so informacije o lastnostih oziroma identiteti dražljajev ter njihovemu vrstnem redu v spominu predstavljene in procesirane ločeno, tako pri prepoznavi, kot pri priklicu. Ločeno procesiranje obeh vrst informacij podpirajo ne le vedenjske, temveč tudi številne slikovne študije. Kako se kodirajo informacije o vrstnem redu, skuša pojasniti več teoretskih modelov. V tem članku bomo pregledali štiri skupine modelov, ki temeljijo na mehanizmu meddražljajskih povezav oziroma verižnega kodiranja, mehanizmu neposrednega ali direktnega kodiranja, mehanizmu hierarhičnega kodiranja ter mehanizmu velikostnega kodiranja. Modeli se razlikujejo tako glede vedenjskih napovedi kot tudi glede predpostavljenih živčnih mehanizmov, na katerih temeljijo. Vsak izmed predlaganih mehanizmov pri tem uspešno pojasnjuje nekatere vidike procesiranja vrstnega reda, nobenemu pa ne uspe pojasniti vseh obstoječih spoznanj in rezultatov študij vrstnega reda. Spomin za zaporedje predstavlja kompleksen proces, zato je verjetno, da je vrstni red kodiran in procesiran s pomočjo več kot le enega mehanizma. Obstoječa spoznanja in modeli puščajo obilico odprtih vprašanj ter nakazujejo možnost obstoja več različnih vrst spomina za vrstni red informacij.
Ključne besede: delovni spomin, kodiranje vrstnega reda, kognitivni mehanizmi
Citiraj:
Dolenc, B. in Repovš, G. (2014). Kodiranje informacije o vrstnem redu v delovnem spominu: pregled vedenjskih raziskav in kognitivnih mehanizmov [Order information coding in working memory: Review of behavioural studies and cognitive mechanisms]. Psihološka obzorja, 23, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.20419/2014.23.391
Seznam literature v članku
Acheson, D. J. in MacDonald, M. C. (2009). Verbal working memory and language production: Common approaches to the serial ordering of verbal information. Psychological Bulletin, 135(1), 50–68. CrossRef
Agam, Y., Huang, J. in Sekuler, R. (2010). Neural correlates of sequence encoding in visuomotor learning. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103(3), 1418–1424. CrossRef
Anderson, J. R. in Matessa, M. (1997). A production system theory of serial memory. Psychological Review, 104(4), 728–748. CrossRef
Awh, E., Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., Schumacher, E. H., Koeppe, R. A. in Katz, S. (1996). Dissociation of storage and rehearsal in verbal working memory: Evidence from positron emission tomography. Psychological Science, 7(1), 25–31. CrossRef
Baddeley, A. (1968). How does acoustic similarity influence short-term memory? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20(3), 249–264. CrossRef
Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford, Velika Britanija: Clarendon Press.
Baddeley, A. (2002). Is working memory still working? European Psychologist, 7(2), 85–97. CrossRef
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(10), 829–839. CrossRef
Baddeley, A. (2007). Working memory, thought, and action. Oxford, Velika Britanija: Oxford University Press. CrossRef
Bjork, E. J. in Healy, A. F. (1974). Short-term order and item retention. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13(1), 80–97. CrossRef
Botvinick, M. M. in Plaut, D. C. (2006). Short-term memory for serial order: A recurrent neural network model. Psychological Review, 113(2), 201–233. CrossRef
Brown, G. D. A., Morin, C. in Lewandowsky, S. (2006). Evidence for time-based models of free recall. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13(4), 717–723. CrossRef
Brown, G. D. A., Preece, T. in Hulme, C. (2000). Oscillator-based memory for serial order. Psychological Review, 107(1), 127–181. CrossRef
Burgess, N. in Hitch, G. J. (1992). Toward a network model of the articulatory loop. Journal of Memory and Language, 31(4), 429–460. CrossRef
Burgess, N. in Hitch, G. J. (1999). Memory for serial order: A network model of the phonological loop and its timing. Psychological Review, 106(3), 551–581. CrossRef
Cabeza, R., Mangels, J., Nyberg, L., Habib, R., Houle, S., McIntosh, A. R. in Tulving, E. (1997). Brain regions differentially involved in remembering what and when: a PET study. Neuron, 19(4), 863–870. CrossRef
Cabeza, R., Nichole, N. D., Houle, S., Mangels, J. A. in Nyberg, L. (2000). Age-related differences in neural activity during item and temporal-order memory retrieval: A positron emission tomography study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(1), 197–206. CrossRef
Cantor, J. in Engle, R. W. (1993). Working-memory capacity as long-term memory activation: An individual-differences approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19(5), 1101–1114. CrossRef
Carpenter, A. F., Georgopoulos, A. P. in Pellizzer, G. (1999). Motor cortical encoding of serial order in a context-recall task. Science, 283(5408), 1752–1757. CrossRef
Carretti, B., Borella, E., Cornoldi, C. in De Beni, R. (2009). Role of working memory in explaining the performance of individuals with specific reading comprehension difficulties: A meta-analysis. Learning and Individual Differences, 19(2), 246–251. CrossRef
Conrad, R. (1965). Order error in immediate recall of sequences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 4(3), 161–169. CrossRef
Cowan, N. (1993). Activation, attention, and short-term memory. Memory & Cognition, 21(2), 162–167. CrossRef
Crowder, R. G. (1968). Evidence for the chaining hypothesis of serial verbal learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76(4), 497–500. CrossRef
Daneman, M. in Merikle, P. M. (1996). Working memory and language comprehension: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 3(4), 422–433. CrossRef
Dell, G. S., Burger, L. K. in Svec, W. R. (1997). Language production and serial order: A functional analysis and a model. Psychological Review, 104(1), 123–147. CrossRef
D'Esposito, M., Detre, J. A., Alsop, D. C., Shin, R. K., Atlas, S. in Grossman, M. (1995). The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory. Nature, 378(6554), 279–281. CrossRef
D'Esposito, M., Postle, B. R., Ballard, D. in Lease, J. (1999). Maintenance versus manipulation of information held in working memory: An event-related fMRI study. Brain and Cognition, 41(1), 66–86. CrossRef
Engelkamp, J. in Dehn, D. M. (2000). Item and order information in subject-performed tasks and experimenter-performed tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(3), 671–682. CrossRef
Farrell, S. (2008). Multiple roles for time in short-term memory: Evidence from serial recall of order and timing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(1), 128–145. CrossRef
Farrell, S. in Lewandowsky, S. (2003). Dissimilar items benefit from phonological similarity in serial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(5), 838–849. CrossRef
Gathercole, S. E. in Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language-disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29(3), 336–360. CrossRef
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1995). Architecture of the prefrontal cortex and the central executive. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 769(1), 71–83. CrossRef
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1996). The prefrontal landscape: Implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 351(1346), 1445–1453. CrossRef
Grossberg, S. (1978). Behavioral contrast in short-term memory: Serial binary memory models or parallel continuous memory models? Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 17(3), 199–219. CrossRef
Gupta, P. in MacWhinney, B. (1997). Vocabulary acquisition and verbal short-term memory: Computational and neural bases. Brain and Language, 59(2), 267–333. CrossRef
Henderson, L. in Matthews, M. L. (1970). Perception and memory loss of item and order information in short-term memory. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 9(2), 231–233. CrossRef
Henson, R. N. A. (1998). Short-term memory for serial order: The start-end model. Cognitive Psychology, 36(2), 73–137. CrossRef
Henson, R. N. A. (1999a). Coding position in short-term memory. International Journal of Psychology, 34(5), 403–409. CrossRef
Henson, R. N. A. (1999b). Positional information in short-term memory: Relative or absolute? Memory & Cognition, 27(5), 915–927. CrossRef
Henson, R. N. A. (2001). Serial order in short-term memory. The Psychologist, 14(2), 70–73.
Henson, R. N. A., Burgess, N. in Frith, C. D. (2000). Recoding, storage, rehearsal and grouping in verbal short-term memory: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 38(4), 426–440. CrossRef
Henson, R. N. A., Hartley, T., Burgess, N., Hitch, G. in Flude, B. (2003). Selective interference with verbal short-term memory for serial order information: A new paradigm and tests of a timing signal hypothesis. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 56(8), 1307–1334.
Henson, R. N. A., Norris, D. G., Page, M. P. A. in Baddeley, A. D. (1996). Unchained memory: Error patterns rule out chaining models of immediate serial recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 49(1), 80–115. CrossRef
Johnson, G. J. (1991). A distinctiveness model of serial learning. Psychological Review, 98(2), 204–217. CrossRef
Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., Mitchell, K. J., Greene, E. J. in Anderson, A. W. (2003). fMRI evidence for organization of prefrontal cortex by both type of process and type of information. Cerebral Cortex, 13(3), 265–273. CrossRef
Jonides, J., Lacey, S. C. in Nee, D. E. (2005). Processes of working memory in brain and mind. Current Directions Psychological Science, 14(1), 2–5. CrossRef
Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., Marshuetz, C., Koeppe, R. A. in Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (1998). Inhibition in verbal working memory revealed by brain activation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95(14), 8410–8413. CrossRef
Just, M. A. in Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99(1), 122–149. CrossRef
Kahana, M. J. in Jacobs, J. (2000). Interresponse times in serial recall: Effects of intraserial repetition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(5), 1188–1197. CrossRef
Kane, M. J. in Engle, R. W. (2002). The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention and general fluid intelligence: An individual differences perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9(4), 637–671. CrossRef
Keele, S. W., Ivry, R., Mayr, U., Hazeltine, E. in Heuer, H. (2003). The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation. Psychological Review, 110(2), 316–339. CrossRef
Kyllonen, P. C. in Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity?!. Intelligence, 14(4), 389–433. CrossRef
Leclercq, A.-L. in Majerus, S. (2010). Serial-order short-term memory predicts vocabulary development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 46(2), 417–427. CrossRef
Lee, C. L. in Estes, W. K. (1981). Item and order information in short-term memory: Evidence for multilevel perturbation processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory7(3), 149–169. CrossRef
Marshuetz, C. (2005). Order information in working memory: An integrative review of evidence from brain and behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 131(3), 323–339. CrossRef
Marshuetz, C. in Smith, E. E. (2006). Working memory for order information: Multiple cognitive and neural mechanisms. Neuroscience, 139(1), 195–200. CrossRef
Marshuetz, C., Smith, E. E., Jonides, J., DeGutis, J. in Chenevert, T. L. (2000). Order information in working memory: fMRI evidence for parietal and prefrontal mechanisms. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(2), 130–144. CrossRef
McElree, B. in Dosher, B. A. (1993). Serial retrieval processes in the recovery of order information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3), 291–315. CrossRef
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63(2), 81–97. CrossRef
Moyer, R. S. in Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgments of numerical inequality. Nature, 215(5109), 1519–1520.
Mulligan, N. W. (1999). The effects of perceptual interference at encoding on organization and order: Investigating the roles of item-specific and relational information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(1), 54–69. CrossRef
Murdock, B. B. (1993). TODAM2: A model for the storage and retrieval of item, associative, and serial order information. Psychological Review, 100(2), 183–203. CrossRef
Nairne, J. S. (1991). Positional uncertainty in long-term memory. Memory & Cognition, 19(4), 332–340. CrossRef
Nairne, J. S. (1992). The loss of positional certainty in long-term memory. Psychological Science, 3(3), 199–202. CrossRef
Neath, I. (1993). Distinctiveness and serial position effects in recognition. Memory & Cognition, 21(5), 689–698. CrossRef
Neath, I. in Crowder, R. G. (1990). Schedules of presentation and temporal distinctiveness in human memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(2), 316–327. CrossRef
Owen, A. M., McMillan, K. M., Laird, A. R. in Bullmore, E. (2005). N-back working memory paradigm: A meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 25(1), 46–59. CrossRef
Page, M. P. A. in Norris, D. (1998). The primacy model: A new model of immediate serial recall. Psychological Review, 105(4), 761–781. CrossRef
Salthouse, T. A. (1993). Influence of working memory on adult age differences in matrix reasoning. British Journal of Psychology, 84(2), 171–199. CrossRef
Schuck, N. W., Gaschler, R., Keisler, A. in Frensch, P. A. (2012). Position-item associations play a role in the acquisition of order knowledge in an implicit serial reaction time task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(2), 440–456. CrossRef
Shiffrin, R. M. in Cook, J. R. (1978). Short-term forgetting of item and order information. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17(2), 189–218. CrossRef
Smith, E. E. in Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 283(5408), 1657–1661. CrossRef
Smith, E. E., Jonides, J. in Koeppe, R. A. (1996). Dissociating verbal and spatial working memory using PET. Cerebral Cortex, 6(1), 11–20. CrossRef
Sternberg, S. (1966). High-Speed Scanning in Human Memory. Psychonomic Science, 8(2), 55–56. CrossRef
Sternberg, S. (1967). Retrieval of contextual information from human memory. Science, 153(3736), 652–654. CrossRef
Wickelgren, W. A. (1967). Rehearsal grouping and hierarchical organization of serial position cues in short-term memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 19(2), 97–102. CrossRef
Young, R. K. (1962). Tests of three hypotheses about the effective stimulus in serial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(3), 307–313. CrossRef
Zheng, X., Swanson, H. L. in Marcoulides, G. A. (2011). Working memory components as predictors of children's mathematical word problem solving. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110(4), 481–498. CrossRef